Effects of Age and Initial Risk Perception on Balloon Analog ...

文章推薦指數: 80 %
投票人數:10人

Balloon Analog Risk Task. To measure risky behavior, a version of the BART (Lejuez et al., 2002) programmed in Java Script language (interface ... ThisarticleispartoftheResearchTopic Theagingdecision-maker:Advancesinunderstandingtheimpactofcognitivechangeondecision-making Viewall 8 Articles Articles EmmaV.Ward MiddlesexUniversity,UnitedKingdom ThomasHess NorthCarolinaStateUniversity,UnitedStates LoreenMamerow UniversityofBasel,Switzerland Theeditorandreviewers'affiliationsarethelatestprovidedontheirLoopresearchprofilesandmaynotreflecttheirsituationatthetimeofreview. Abstract Introduction MaterialsandMethods Results Discussion Conclusion AuthorContributions Funding ConflictofInterestStatement Acknowledgment References SuggestaResearchTopic> DownloadArticle DownloadPDF ReadCube EPUB XML(NLM) Supplementary Material Exportcitation EndNote ReferenceManager SimpleTEXTfile BibTex totalviews ViewArticleImpact SuggestaResearchTopic> SHAREON OpenSupplementalData ORIGINALRESEARCHarticle Front.Psychol.,06May2016 |https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00659 EffectsofAgeandInitialRiskPerceptiononBalloonAnalogRiskTask:TheMediatingRoleofProcessingSpeedandNeedforCognitiveClosure MaciejKoscielniak,KlaraRydzewskaandGrzegorzSedek* InterdisciplinaryCenterforAppliedCognitiveStudies,DepartmentofPsychology,SWPSUniversityofSocialSciencesandHumanities,Warsaw,Poland Accordingtothedual-processtheoreticalperspectiveadoptedinthepresentedresearch,theefficiencyofdeliberativeprocessesindecisionmakingdeclineswithage,butexperientialprocessesarerelativelywell-preserved.Theage-relateddifferencesindeliberativeandexperientialprocessesinriskydecision-makingwereexaminedinthisresearchbyapplyingtheBalloonAnalogRiskTask(BART).Weanalyzedtheinfluenceofageonriskacceptanceanddecision-makingperformanceintwoagegroupsoffemaleparticipants(youngeradults,n=81;olderadults,n=76),withadditionalexperimentalmanipulationofinitialriskperception.WepredictedandconfirmedthatagingsignificantlyworsensperformanceonthebehavioralBARTmeasuresduetoage-relateddeclineindeliberativeprocesses.OlderparticipantswerefoundtoexhibitsignificantlyhigherriskaversionandlowerBARTperformance,andtheeffectofagewasmediatedbycognitive(processingspeed)andmotivational(needforcognitiveclosure)mechanisms.Moreover,olderadultsadapttotheinitialfailure(vs.success)similarly,asyoungeradultsduetopreservedefficiencyofexperientialprocesses.Theseresultssuggestfuturedirectionsforminimizingnegativeeffectsofaginginriskydecision-makingandindicatecompensatoryprocesses,whicharepreservedduringaging. Introduction Thecommonunderstandingisthatyoungpeoplearepronetorisktaking,whileolderadultsareusedasexamplesofcautionandriskaversion(Rolisonetal.,2012).However,thosestereotypesdonotfindsupportinnumerousresearchresultsandcorrelationaldata(Droretal.,1998;Chouetal.,2007;Samanez-Larkinetal.,2011).Manydataindicatetheoppositetendency:peopleaged55yearsormoreareactivelymakinginvestmentchoicesthatinvolvelargeamountsofriskacceptance(Brown,2002),andtheyalsooccupythehighest-rankingmanagerialpositionsamongS&P500companies(StatisticBrainResearchInstitute,2015). Someresearchers(Chaubey,1974;Deakinetal.,2004)hypothesizegradualage-relateddeclineinqualityoffunctioningwithinriskanduncertaintyconditions,whichinturnleadstodeficitsindecision-makingefficiency.However,otherpsychologicalscientistsdonotagreewiththisnotionandreportevidencethatnotonlysupportsnodifferencesbetweenthequalityofolderandyoungeradults’choicesinriskanduncertaintyconditions(Droretal.,1998),butalsoclaimsthatthesegroupsarecharacterizedbysimilardecisionstrategieswhenfacedwithriskychoices(Kovalchiketal.,2005;BruinedeBruinetal.,2014). Becausetheissueofagedifferencesinriskychoiceisaverycomplexone,anincreasingamountofresearchisfocusedonspecifyingdecisiontaskcontextsandsettingsthatdeterminewhetherolderadultsseekoravoidrisk(forrecentreviews,seeHenningeretal.,2010;Mataetal.,2011;Cavanaghetal.,2012).Weappliedthedual-processperspectiveofageeffectsondecision-making,recentlyelaboratedbyHess(2015),foracontextualtheoreticalmodel.Classicaldual-processmodels(e.g.,Bargh,1994;Epstein,1994;Kahneman,2003)differentiatethedeliberativeprocesses(effortful,analytical,oftenverbal,andslow)fromtheexperientialprocesses(automatic,associative,oftenaffective,andfast).Thisdistinctionisimportantbecausetheempiricalevidence(forreviews,seePetersetal.,2007;Hess,2015)showsthatindecision-making,deliberativeprocessesareimpairedbyaging,butexperientialprocessesarerelativelywell-preserved.Inthisresearch,weexaminedage-relateddifferencesbetweendeliberativeandexperientialprocessesinriskydecision-makingbyapplyingtheBalloonAnalogRiskTask(BART;Lejuezetal.,2002).Meta-analysisbyMataetal.(2011)indicatesthatriskytaskssuchastheIowaGamblingTask(IGT),theBehavioralInvestmentAllocationStrategy(BIAS),andtheBART,whichinvolvemakingdecisionsfromexperience(i.e.,riskinformationisnotexplicitlypresented;participantsestimateriskinanumberoftrials),showedstrikinglydifferentage-relateddifferences.Morespecifically,intheIGTandBIAS,olderadults,ratherthanyoungerones,weremorerisk-seeking.Incontrast,intheBART,olderadultsweremorerisk-aversethantheiryoungercounterparts. Therefore,theBARTmightbecurrentlyassessedasthe“prototype”taskinwhicholderadultspreferrisk-avoidingstrategies.However,theresearchonage-relateddifferencesintheBARTarescarce,andweidentifiedonlytwostudiesthatdirectlycompareddifferencesbetweenolderandyoungeradults.Theyyieldedonlypartiallyconsistentfindings.TheresearchbyHenningeretal.(2010)demonstratedclearage-relatedimpairmentsinthemainbehavioralmeasures(i.e.,adjustednumberofpumpsandnumberofexplosions)intheBART.However,intheresearchbyRolisonetal.(2012),age-relateddifferencesinbehavioralmeasureswereabsent,butthecognitivemodelingshowedsomesubtledifferences(olderadultsweremorecautious)intheinitialperceptionsofrisk.Obviously,moreresearchisneededtoexaminethemechanismsofage-relateddifferencesintheBART.Thefirstgoalofourresearchwastounderstandhowage-relateddeclineincognitivedeliberativeskill,suchasprocessingspeed,affectsBARTperformance.Thesecondgoalwastoexaminewhetherolderadultsshowarelativepreservationofexperientialprocesses(affectiveresilience,Petersetal.,2007;Hess,2015)andareasadaptiveasyoungeradultsinadjustingtoaseriesofincidentalfailuresintheBART.Inthenextsections,theBARTwillbesuccinctlypresented;wewillthenreviewtheexistingevidenceofage-relateddifferencesindeliberativeandexperientialprocessestopredicttheirrolesintherelationshipbetweenageandBARTperformance. UsingthecomputerizedBART,participantsaretopumpaseriesofvirtualballoons,andwitheachpump,pointsaregained.Participantsdecidewhethertocontinueorstoppumpingandindicatethisbyclickingonabuttononthemonitor.Ifpumpingisterminated,thepointsgainedinthattrialwillbesecurelystoredina“bank.”Pumpingtheballoontoomanytimescarriestheriskoftheballoonexploding,andthepointsgainedinthattrialarelost.However,iftheballoonisnotpumpedenough,theamountofpointsearnedissmall.Therefore,risktakingisrewardedonlytoacertainpoint,andthenitispenalized.Thisisareflectionofnumerousreal-lifesituations. TheBARThasbeenproventohavestrongecologicalvaliditybecauseitisastrongpredictorofreal-liferisktakingbehaviorinanumberofpopulations(Lejuezetal.,2003;Aklinetal.,2005;Hopkoetal.,2006;Bornovalovaetal.,2009),includingolderadults(Henningeretal.,2010;Cavanaghetal.,2012;Rolisonetal.,2012;Seamanetal.,2015).Lejuezetal.(2002)claimedthattheBARTissometimesamorereliablemeasureofrisk-takingbehaviorthanself-reportsanddemographicdata.Accordingtoameta-analysisinvolvingtheBART(Lauriolaetal.,2014),mostpublicationsdemonstratedsignificantcorrelationsbetweentheaverageadjustednumberofpumpsandriskybehaviorsinreallife,suchassmoking,alcoholanddruguse,aggression,participationinunprotectedsex,andgambling. Age-relatedlimitationsindeliberativecognitiveprocessesmightbeplausiblemechanismsofimpairedBARTperformanceamongolderadults.WhenconfrontedwithcomplexriskydecisiontaskssuchastheBART,effectivefunctioningofparticipantsdemandsactivedeliberation,suchascountingandaveragingthenumberofpumpsacrosstrials,suppressingnegativefeelingscausedbyballoonexplosions,andweighingtheprosandconsoffurtherpumping.Hence,therearehighdemandsoncognitiveandattentionalresourceswhenitcomestoeffectiveBARTperformance.Thescientificliterature(Engleetal.,2005;ThorntonandDumke,2005;Salthouse,2006,2012;Sedeketal.,2013),indicatesaclearandmarkedmonotonicdecreaseinbasicfluidcognitiveabilities(suchasprocessingspeed,workingmemorycapacity,andfluidintelligence)fromearlyadulthoodthroughmiddleagetooldage,andthisisresponsiblefordecreasedperformanceinvariouscognitivetasks.TheprocessingspeedtheorydevelopedbySalthouse(1996)andusingtheDigitSymbolSubstitutionTest(DSST)asaprincipalmeasure,accountsforage-relatedchanges,suchasimpairmentinlogicalreasoning(Salthouse,2001;SedekandvonHecker,2004),inmanycognitivetasks.UsingtheBART,Henningeretal.(2010)foundthatolderadultshavegreateraversiontoriskthanyoungeradults,thusthequalityoftheirdecision-makinginthattaskwassignificantlylower.Inaddition,theyshowedthatage-relateddifferencesinBARTperformancearemediatedbyprocessingspeed.However,cognitivespeedwasmeasuredusingtheDigitSymboltest,whichissimilartotheDSSTbuthassomedifferentcharacteristics.Theprocessingspeedtheory(withDSSTasaprincipalmeasure)developedbySalthouse(1996)accountsforage-relateddifferencesinmanycognitivetasks.Accordingtothistheory,slowerspeedofexecutingcognitiveoperationsinolderageimpairsperformanceinmorecomplextasksbecauseoftwomechanisms.Thekeydefiningfeatureofthefirstmechanism(the“timelimited”mechanism)isthattimeconstraintscauseprocessingoftheinitialinformationtobeincompleteanddefective.Thisisincontrasttothesecondmechanism(the“simultaneity”mechanism)inwhichprocessingtheinitialinformationiscompletebutdecaycausesittobecomeunavailableinthelaterstagesofprocessing.Bothcognitivemechanismsmayberesponsibleforage-relateddeclineinBARTperformance.Hence,toreplicateandextendpreviousfindingsabouttheinfluenceofageonBARTperformance(Henningeretal.,2010),theDSSTwasincludedasameasureofprocessingspeed. Anotherwayinwhicholderadultsappeartomanagelowerefficiencyindeliberativecognitiveprocessesisthroughselectiveengagementincognitiveresources.Olderadultsmayconserveresourcesbysimplifyingtheirinteractionswiththeenvironmentandbylimitingboththequantityandcomplexityoftheinformationtowhichtheyattend.Thisdescriptionisbasedontheclassicalframeworkofselection,optimization,andcompensation(SOC;BaltesandBaltes,1990).AccordingtoHess(2000,2014,2015),agingisconnectedwithincreasingcostsofengagementineffortfulcognitiveactivities.Asagingproceeds,moreresourcesarenecessarytoachieveaparticularlevelofperformanceinaneffortfultask.Furthermore,aperceptionofcostsresultsinlackofintrinsicmotivationtocommitresourcestocognitivelydemandingtasks,andthisinturnisreflectedintheselectionprocessesofdirectingandenergizing.WemeasuredcognitivemotivationbyapplyingtheNeedforCognitiveClosureShortScale(NFCC;Kruglanski,2004).Theneedforclosurehasbeendefinedasadesireforadefiniteanswertoaquestionorforanyfirmanswerratherthanuncertainty,confusion,orambiguity(KruglanskiandWebster,1996).AgingusuallyincreasesscoresontheNFCC;however,ageeffectshavevariedsizeeffectandaredivergentforthedifferentsubscales(Blanchard-Fieldsetal.,2001;Cornelisetal.,2009;Kossowskaetal.,2012b).ThemostconsistentageeffectsareobservedfortwoNFCCsubscales:DesireforPredictabilityandPreferenceforOrderandStructure(Blanchard-Fieldsetal.,2001;Czarneketal.,2015).MostpreviousresearchhasdemonstratedthattheNFCCisrelatedtoasimplifiedcognitiveprocessinvolvedindecision-making,alimitedinformationsearch,andapreferenceforclearandunambiguousjudgment(seetherecentreviewinRoetsetal.,2015).Severalauthors(Cornelisetal.,2009;Kossowskaetal.,2012b;Roetsetal.,2015)suggestedtheinterplaybetweenage-relateddeclineindeliberativeprocessesandage-relatedincreaseintheNFCC.Namely,thecomplexityofencounteredtasksinreal-worldsituationsoftenexceedsthecognitiveresourcesofolderadults,andthiscorrespondstotheage-relatedincreaseinsomeoftheNFCCsubscalesmeasuringepistemicmotivation(i.e.,theyaremotivatedtostriveforsimplification,morepredictability,andorder).Ourrecentstudy(Czarneketal.,2015)showedsomesupportforthedoublemediationofageeffectonstereotypicjudgmentsbycognitiveandmotivationalprocesses.Namely,wefoundthat,comparedtoyoungerparticipants,olderadultsweremorelikelytorelyonstereotypicinferenceswhentheyreadastoryaboutout-groupmembers.Inaddition,thefindingsshowedthatmakingmorestereotypicalinferencesbyoldervs.youngeradultsinrelationtoout-groupmemberswasmediatedbycognitive(TrailMakingTest)andmotivational(NFCC)mechanisms.TheimportantresearchquestioniswhethertheinfluenceofagingonsomemeasuresoftheBARTisalsoindependentlydeterminedbyadeclineinmentalspeedandanincreaseintheNFCC. Recentfindings(e.g.,QueenandHess,2010;Hessetal.,2012)andreviews(Petersetal.,2007;HessandEnnis,2014;Hess,2015)showedthatage-relateddifferencesindecisionmakingtasksareeliminatedwhendecisionmakingperformancestronglyreliesonexperiential(e.g.,affective)processes.Interestingly,asubstantialnumberofstudiesontheIGT(Becharaetal.,1994),reflectingtheactivityofaffectivecorticalstructures,didnotdemonstrateage-relateddifferences(e.g.,MacPhersonetal.,2002;Kovalchiketal.,2005;Woodetal.,2005;butseeclearage-relateddifferencesinDenburgetal.,2009).Themostinspiringforuswerethedetailedanalysesofage-relateddifferencesfoundbyWoodetal.(2005).Theyfoundthatyoungeradultsappliedbothdeliberativeandexperientialprocessesinformingtheirchoicesbetweenmore-andless-riskycarddecksintheIGT.However,olderadults’choicebehaviorswereguidedmorebyrecentlossesandgains,suggestinggreateremploymentofexperientialprocesses.ThisstrategywasquiteeffectiveinthisstudybecausebotholderandyoungeradultsperformedsimilarlyintheIGT. InspiredbytheseanalysesoftheIGTbyWoodetal.(2005),wedecidedtoexperimentallymanipulateexperiencedfailuresandsuccessesinpreliminarytrialsoftheBART.Specifically,weprocedurallyincreasedtheparticipant’sinitialfailures(thefirstthreeballoonsinthetaskexplodealmostimmediately,andparticipantshardlyearnanypoints)orinitialsuccesses(thefirstthreeballoonsinthetaskexplodeaftermorethan20pumps,henceparticipantsearnedalotofpoints).AsseenbyWoodandassociates,suchmanipulationshouldbeguidedbyexperientialprocessesandaffectinbothyoungerandolderparticipantssimilarly.Accordingtorecentresearchapplyingadvancedcognitivemodelingtoexplainage-relateddifferencesinBARTperformance(Cavanaghetal.,2012;Rolisonetal.,2012),suchmanipulationshouldinitiallysignificantlydifferentiateadopteddecisionstrategies(morecautiousafterinitialfailures,moreriskyafterinitialsuccesses),butthiseffectshouldbeofshortduration(withincreasedexperience). TheimpactofexperientialprocessesinBARTperformanceisrelatedtothepotentialroleofpositiveemotionsinexplainingage-relateddifferencesindecision-making.Improvedaffectiveexperiencehasbeenfoundtobeassociatedwithaging(Carstensen,2006;CharlesandCarstensen,2010;Sedeketal.,2014).Moreover,studiesshowthataffectinfluencessearchtendenciesandtherefore,decision-making.Asanimportantexample,increasedpositivemoodamongolderadultsleadstolessinformationsearchinsequentialchoicetasks(vonHelversenandMata,2012),therebyworseningperformance.BecausetheBARTisundoubtedlyasequentialdecision-makingtask,wealsoexaminedwhetherpositiveaffectinfluencesBARTperformance. IntheBART,womentendtopumptheballoonsubstantiallylessthanmen(Lejuezetal.,2002).Toeliminatecomplexinteractionsthatmaybeevokedbytheunexpectedinfluenceofparticipants’sex,wedecidedtoconductthisstudyusingagroupofonlywomen,thosecommonlyregardedasmoreriskaversethanmen. Tosuccinctlysummarizeourmainpredictions:(1)AgingsignificantlyworsensperformanceonbehavioralBARTmeasuresbecauseofage-relateddeclineindeliberativeprocesses;(2)Experiencingincidentalfailures(vs.successes)atthebeginningofthetaskaffectsBARTperformance(atleastinthefirstphaseofthetask).However,olderandyoungeradultsshouldadaptsimilarlytothismanipulationbecauseofpreservedefficiencyofexperientialprocesses. MaterialsandMethods Participants One-hundredfifty-eightfemaleparticipantsfromtwospecificagegroupswereinvitedtoparticipateinthestudy.Thegroupofyoungeradultsconsistedof81studentsaged18–23years(M=19.86years;SD=1.00)recruitedfromfirst-andsecond-yearuniversitycoursesinexchangeforpartialcredit.Thegroupofolderadultsincluded77participantsaged65–80years(M=69.19years;SD=4.32)whovolunteeredtoparticipatethrougharecruitmentcompany.ParticipantswhoperformedbestontheBARTreceivedmaterialprizes,namelyoptionalbooksorshoppingvouchersoftheequalnominalvalueof$12. Inclusioncriteriawerepossessionofabasiclevelofcomputerliteracy,allowingforcomfortableandfreeperformanceofthecomputerizedtaskanduseofcorrectivelenses(eithereyeglassesorcontactlenses)whenvisiondefectswarranteditsothateffectsofpoorvisionwereminimized. Informationabouteducationlevelintermsoftheyearsofformaleducationcompletedwasparticipant-provided,andnosignificantdifferenceswerefoundbetweenthegroups.Amongtheolderadults,mean(SD)levelofeducationwas13.43(3.06)years,whereasamongtheyoungeradults,itwas13.59(2.05)years(F<1). Cognitive,Motivational,andAffectiveMeasures MentalspeedwasmeasuredusingtheDSST(Wechsler,1981),aclassicalmeasureofprocessingspeedthatinvolvessubstitutionofdigitswithsymbolsaccordingtoagiventemplatewithinatimelimit. AnadditionalmeasureoftheabilitytoengageineffortfulinferenceprocessesandavoidjudgmentbiaseswasdeterminedbyapplyingtheCognitiveReflectionTest(CRT;Frederick,2005),athree-itemmeasure(e.g.,“abatandaballcost$1.10intotal;thebatcosts$1.00morethantheball;howmuchdoestheballcost?”). TheNFCC(KruglanskiandWebster,1996)wasmeasuredusingthePolishversion(Kossowskaetal.,2012a).Participantswereaskedtoratethemselveson15itemsdescribingvariousreal-lifesituationsusingaLikertscalerangingfrom1(totallydisagree)to6(totallyagree).Additionally,thePolishversion(FajkowskaandMarszał-Wiśniewska,2009)ofthePositiveandNegativeAffectSchedule–ExpandedForm(PANAS-X;WatsonandClark,1994)wasappliedtomeasurecurrentaffectivestate.Theinstrumentconsistedof40affectivewords(suchaslonely,concentrated,andhappy)andparticipantsratedhowwelleachitemdescribedtheircurrentmoodonaseven-pointscalerangingfrom1(notatall)to7(verymuch).Positiveandnegativeaffectscoreswerecalculatedbytakingthemeanratingofeitherpositiveornegativeitems. BalloonAnalogRiskTask Tomeasureriskybehavior,aversionoftheBART(Lejuezetal.,2002)programmedinJavaScriptlanguage(interfaceshowninFigure1)wasused.Thetaskconsistedoftwoseriescarriedoutapproximately15minapart.Eachcomprised30trials,andineachtrial,theparticipantwaspresentedwitharealisticpictureofaballoononthecomputermonitor.TheconstructionofBARTwasthesameasdescribedindetailbyLejuezetal.(2002).Theseriesweremostsimilartoyellowballoonseries(Lejuezetal.,2002)inwhichtomaximizeearningsparticipantsshouldlearntopumpeachballoonapproximately16times,ignoringtheexplosions.Inourseries,theaveragebreakpointwas15(30trialswiththeexplosionsbetween1and30pumps);therefore,tomaximizeearningsparticipantsshouldlearntopumpeachballoonapproximately15times.Thetaskwastovirtuallyinflatetheballoonbyclickingonthebuttononthemonitormarked‘PUMP’anditwasdoneinaself-pacedmanner.Eachtimethiswasdone,theballoonvisuallygrewinsize,andtheparticipantreceivedonevirtualpoint.Atanytime,theparticipantcouldstopinflatingtheballoonandsecurethepointsinavirtualbank.Thiswasdonebyclickingonabuttonmarked‘COLLECT.’Eachpumpincreasedtheriskoflosingallthepointsgainedinthecurrenttrial,butitdidnotaffectpointsalreadysecuredinthebank.Theballoonwouldexplodeatarandompointintimebetweenthefirstandthirtiethpumps(themaximumnumberofpumpswas30). FIGURE1 FIGURE1.BalloonAnalogRiskTask. ExperimentalManipulationoftheBART WeexperimentallymodifiedtheBARTtoinfluencetheparticipant’sinitialfailuresorsuccesses.Thetaskincludedtwoseriesrequiringinflationof30balloonsineach.Thenumberofpumpsuntilaballoonexplodedwasrandomized(trialswiththeexplosionsbetween1and30pumps)exceptforthefirstthreetrials,whichwereusedfortheexperimentalmanipulation.Thesethreetrialswereeitherunlucky(explosionswithpumps3,4,and1)orlucky(explosionswithpumps25,27,and28),andparticipantsreceivedbothseriesinarandomorderduringthecourseofthetask.Weusedthetwoversionsofrandomordersforthetrials(4–30)andtheseversionswerecounterbalancedacrossparticipantsfortheserieswiththeluckyandunluckytrials.Theeffectivenessofthisemployedmanipulationwasconfirmedusingwithinsubjectsanalysisofvariance,wherethedependentvariablewasthenumberofpointsearnedinthefirstthreetrialsintwoseries(unluckyvs.luckyseries):intheunluckyseries,participantsearnedM=0.14points(SD=0.33),whileintheluckyseries,participantsearnedM=10.62points(SD=4.80),F(1,157)=746.48,p<0.001,η2=0.826.Theeffectivenessofthisemployedmanipulationwasadditionallyconfirmedusingwithinsubjectsanalysisofvariance,wherethedependentvariablewasthepercentageofexplosionsinthefirstthreetrialsintwoseries(unluckyvs.luckyseries):intheunluckyseries,95%ofthefirstthreeballoonsexploded,whileintheluckyseries,6%ofthefirstthreeballoonsexploded,F(1,157)=3645.43,p<0.001,η2=0.959. PrincipalBARTMeasures TheBARTprovidesvarioustypesofdatathatdescriberisktoleranceandpreferreddecision-makingstrategies.Weusedthreeprimarymeasurestoassessrisk-takingbehavior:adjustednumberofpumps,totalpointsearned,andtotalnumberofexplosions.ThesemeasureswereoriginallysuggestedbyLejuezetal.(2002). ThemostcommonlyusedmeasurefromtheBARTistheadjustednumberofpumps,calculatedastheaveragenumberofpumpsontheballoonsthatdidnotexplode.Forthepurposeofstatisticalanalysis,thisisthemostfavorablevariablebecauseincorporatingalltrials(includingthoseinwhichtheballoonexploded)wouldresultinusingtrialswhereparticipantsdidnotmaketheirowndecisions,butwereforcedtostoppumping(Huntetal.,2005).Thus,usingtheadjustednumberofpumpsdescribesarelativelyunbiasedriskattitude(Seamanetal.,2015).Makingadeliberatedecisiontostoppumpingisanexampleofanon-punitiveformofrisk-takingbehavior.Onthecontrary,thetotalnumberofexplosionsrepresentsthefrequencyofmakingadecisionthatexceededtheoptimalrisklevelandwhichwasfollowedbyimmediatepunishment(explosionandlossofpoints;Huntetal.,2005). Thetotalnumberofpointscollectedduringbothseries(excludingtrialsintroducedforexperimentalmanipulation)wasthefinalmeasuredeterminedusingtheBART.Thisdependentvariableisoftentreatedasaneffectivenessindexofdecision-makingstrategybecausemaximizationofearningsrequiresidentifyingtheoptimallevelofrisk.However,itisnotaperfectmeasureofriskpropensitybecauseidenticalearningsmaybegainedbothwhenaparticipantearnslittleforeachballoon,butcausesfewexplosionsandwhensheearnslargebutinfrequentreward,resultinginmanyexplosions. Despitethefactthateachofthesemeasuresdescribesadifferentaspectofdecision-makingunderriskanduncertainty,thereisevidencetheyindependently(inmostexperimentalprocedures)producesimilarfindings(Lejuezetal.,2003). Procedure Allparticipantsgaveinformedconsentforparticipationinstudyprocedures.Testswerecompletedinaroomwith7–9otherparticipantsfromthesameagegroup,whoweresimultaneouslydoingthesetestsinthesameorder.Eachparticipantwasprovidedadesksothatpaper-and-pencilmeasurescouldbecompletedaswellasacomputerequippedwiththeWindows7®operatingsystemanda22-inchmonitor.Monitorsweresetinawaythatpreventedparticipantsfromseeingothermonitorsandfromcommunicatingwithoneanother.Threeexperimenterswerepresentatalltimesduringstudyprocedures,ensuringcloseandindividualmonitoringofstudyparticipants. ThefirstseriesoftheBARTwascompletedusingthecomputer.Followingthis,theDSST,measuringmentalspeedofprocessinginformation,wascompleted.ParticipantsweretheninstructedtocompletethePANAS-Xtoself-reportcurrentaffectivestate.TheNFCCandCRTwerethencompleted.Finally,thesecondseriesoftheBARTwascompleted.Afterallparticipantsfinishedthecourseofactivities,thosewhoscoredthemostpointsontheBARTineachgroupreceivedaprize,whichwasmadeexplicittoparticipantspriortocompletionofstudyprocedures.Theentirestudytook30–50mintocomplete. Results TheeffectsofAgeonprocessingspeed,CognitiveReflectionTest,NeedforCognitiveClosure,andPANASXScalearesummarizedinTable1.PreliminaryanalysesshowedthattheOrderofSeries(unluckyfirst,luckyfirst;betweensubjectsvariable)didnotyieldanysignificantresultsasconcernmaineffectandinteractioneffectswithanyothervariablesforalldependentmeasures,henceweomittedthisvariableinfactorialdesign. TABLE1 TABLE1.Participantcharacteristics. RiskTaking Asseeninpreviousstudies,theprincipalmeasureofrisk-takingbehaviorusingtheBARTwastheadjustednumberofpumps;twoadditionalmeasuresweretotalpointsearnedandnumberofexplosions. A2×2:(Age[youngeradults,olderadults]×FirstTrials[unlucky,lucky;withinsubjectvariable])mixedanalysisofvariance(ANOVA)ofthesedependentmeasuresyieldedonlythemaineffectsofAge(seeTable2)andthesystematicmaineffectofFirstTrials(seeTable3). TABLE2 TABLE2.AnalysisofvariancemaineffectsoftheAgevariableonriskindicatorsinBARTtask(2×2schema:Age×FirstTrials). TABLE3 TABLE3.AnalysisofvariancemaineffectsofFirstTrialvariableonriskindicatorsinBARTtask(2×2schema:Age×FirstTrials). ResultsforthemaineffectsoftheAgevariableclearlyindicatethatolderfemaleadultsadopthigherriskavoidancebehaviorthanyoungerfemaleadults,performingworseontheBART(averageadjustednumberofpumpswaslower,fewerpointsearned,lowernumberofexplosions).ThemaineffectsofFirstTrialsconsistentlyindicatethatbotholderandyoungerfemaleadultsadoptedmuchriskierbehaviorinBARTperformancewhenthefirsttrialsintheserieswerelucky(consequentlyaverageadjustednumberofpumpswashigher,morepointsearned,highernumberofexplosions)ratherthanwhentheywereunlucky. AdaptationstoUnluckyandLuckySeries Todetermineinmoredetailtheinfluenceofintroducedexperimentalmanipulation,wecarriedoutamixedmodelANOVAtocomparethebehaviorsofparticipantsimmediatelyaftermanipulation(atthebeginningoftheseries),inthemiddle,andattheendoftheseries.Toaccomplishthis,anadditionalwithinsubjectvariable(PhaseofSeries)wascreated,andtheaverageadjustednumberofpumpsininthreesubsequentphasesofninetrialsfollowingexperimentalmanipulation(firstphase:trials4–12;middlephase:trials13–21;finalphase:trials22–30)wasdetermined. A2×2×3:(Age[youngeradults,olderadults]×FirstTrials[unlucky,lucky;withinsubjectvariable]×PhaseofSeries[first,middle,final;withinsubjectvariable])mixedANOVAoftheadjustednumberofpumpsinninetrialsyieldedtwomaineffectsandoneinteractioneffect. AmaineffectofAgewasfound,F(1,154)=16.60,p<0.001,ηp2=0.097,showingthatadjustednumberofpumpswaslowerinolderadults(M=7.87)thaninyoungeradults(M=9.64).AmaineffectofFirstTrialswasalsofound,F(1,154)=100.82,p<0.001,ηp2=0.396,showingthatadjustednumberofpumpswaslowerwhentheunluckyserieswaspresented(M=7.76)thanwhentheluckyserieswaspresented(M=9.76). ThemaineffectofFirstTrialswasqualifiedassignificantinteractioneffect,namelyFirstTrials×PhaseofSeries,F(1,154)=42.95,p<0.001,ηp2=0.218(seeFigure2–forcompleteness,theageeffectisincluded).AsshowninFigure2,theadjustednumberofpumpsduringtheinitiallyunluckyserieswaslowestinthefirstphaseandthengraduallyincreased(p<0.05)inthesubsequentphases.However,theadjustednumberofpumpsintheinitiallyluckyserieswashigherinthefirstphasethanintheotherphases(p<0.001)andsimilarinthemiddleandfinalphases.Atthebeginningofaseries,theadjustednumberofpumpswashigheriftheserieswasinitiallyluckythanifitwasinitiallyunlucky(ηp2=0.486).Thosedifferencesweregraduallydecreasing(ηp2=0.149inthemiddlephase;ηp2=0.114inthefinalphase).However,allthedifferencesintheadjustednumberofpumpsbetweenluckyandunluckyseriesacrossallphaseswerehighlysignificant(p<0.001). FIGURE2 FIGURE2.AdjustedaveragenumberofpumpsasafunctionofFirstTrials,PhaseofSeries,andAge.Errorbarsrepresentstandarderrors. Verysimilarresultswerefoundwhenweanalyzedtotalnumberofexplosionsexperienced(andpointsearned)inthefirst,middle,andfinalphases;therefore,wedonotreporttheseresultstoavoidredundancyofinformation. Theageofthestudyparticipantsdoesnotstatisticallysignificantlyinteractwithanyoftheothervariables.Therefore,itcanbeconcludedthatagingdoesnotresultinsignificantimpairmentonBARTperformanceafterincidentalfailureorincidentalsuccessexperience. MediatingMechanisms Preliminarycorrelationalanalysesshowedthatonlyprocessingspeed(measuredbyDSST)andneedforcognitiveclosure(NFCC)weresignificantlycorrelatedwithBARTmeasures(Table4);henceonlythosevariableswereconsideredpotentialmediatorsintherelationshipbetweenageandBARTperformance. TABLE4 TABLE4.Correlationtableofdependent,independent,anddemographicvariables. Resultsofthepositiveaffectscalearestatisticallysignificantlydifferentiatedbyage(Table1),confirmingtheexistenceofapositivityeffect(Carstensenetal.,1999).However,positiveaffectdidnotcorrelatewithanymeasuresdeterminedbytheBART(Table4).Similarly,theCRT(Frederick,2005),assumedtomeasureengagementineffortfulinferenceprocesses,hadhigherscoresintheyoungergroup(Table1);howeverthey,too,werenotcorrelatedwithBARTmeasures(Table4). Toverifythehypothesisthatthereisnodirectrelationshipbetweenparticipantageandtheirbehaviorunderriskanduncertainty(aftercontrollingformechanismsexplainingsucharelationship),amediationanalysisusingProcesssoftware(Hayes,2013)wasconducted.Itisimportanttounderlinethatsuchmediationanalysesarenotseenatanyrateascausaldevelopmentalchangebutonlyastheexaminationofdirectandindirectinfluenceofagedifferencesonriskydecisionchoice.AsdescribedbyHayesandScharkow(2013)foroptimaltestsoftheindirecteffectinstatisticalmediationanalysis,weappliedtherecommendedpercentilebootstrapCImethod.TheDSSTscorewasthemeasureofprocessingspeedofcognitivefunctioning,thesumofthetwoNFCCsubscaleswasthemeasureofmotivationtoselectincominginformation. Thestrongestmediatoroftherelationshipbetweenageandriskacceptancemeasuresisprocessingspeed(NFCCwasreliablemediatoronlyfornumberofexplosionsmeasure).InFigure3A,themediationinfluenceofthesevariablesontherelationshipbetweenageandadjustednumberofpumpsisshown.Bootstrapping(n=10000bootstrapresamples,95%confidenceintervals)revealedthesignificantindirecteffectofagingonrisk-seekingviaDSST(b2×b3=-1.86;BootLLCI=-2,91;BootULCI=-0.84),whilethedirecteffectbecomesstatisticallyinsignificantinthismodel(b1=0.80;p=0.29). FIGURE3 FIGURE3.Modelsdepictingmediatedeffectsofagedifferencesviaprocessingspeed(DSST)andNeedforCognitiveClosure(NFCC_2)on:(A)adjustednumberofpumps;(B)totalpoints;(C)numberofexplosionsinBARTexperiment.Boldarrowsindicatesignificantindirecteffects;dashedarrowsindicatenon-significanteffects.Entriesareunstandardizedregressioncoefficientswitht-testvalues.∗p<0.05,∗∗p<0.01,∗∗∗p<0.001. Themediationmechanismisshapedverysimilarlywhenanalyzingtheinfluenceofageontotalscore,expressedasearnedpoints(seeFigure3B),andyieldsanalogousresults:aninsignificantdirecteffectofageonthedependentvariable(b1=5.17;p=0.73)andastrongmediationpathviaprocessingspeed(b2×b3=-35.26;BootLLCI=-58.41;BootULCI=-13.44).Thesefindingsclearlydemonstratethatriskacceptanceandeffectivenessofchoicesmadeunderriskanduncertaintydonotdependdirectlyonage,butontheimpairmentofcognitivefunctions,whicharecharacteristictoaging. ThismodelisevenmoreextensivewhenappliedtotherelationshipbetweenageandthenumberofexplosionsexperiencedintheBART.WhileastrongmaineffectofthisrelationshipisshowninTable2,itbecomesinsignificant(b1=1.16;p=0.58)afterintroducingtwomediatingvariables:theprocessingspeedofinformationandtheneedforclosure(seeFigure3C).TheDSSTisagainamediatingfactor(b2×b3=-3.59;BootLLCI=-6,72;BootULCI=-0.41);however,thescaleofthismediationisadditionallyincreasedbytheNFCC(b4×b5=-1.63;BootLLCI=-3,28;BootULCI=-0.08).Aneedforcognitiveclosureexpressestheepistemicmotivation,whichsignificantlyincreaseswithage,resultinginmorejudiciouspumpingofballoons,thusresultinginfewerexplosions. Discussion ThisstudyusedtheBARTtoinvestigatetheinfluenceofagingonthespecificityofdecision-makingunderriskanduncertaintyamongwomen.Theprimaryresultwasastrongnegativeeffectofageonalldecision-makingaspectsofthetask,affectingboththedegreeofrisktoleranceandefficiencyofperformance.Thisagreeswithpreviousresearchfindingsonage-relatedlimitationsofBARTperformance(Henningeretal.,2010;Cavanaghetal.,2012;Rolisonetal.,2012).Moregenerally,thesefindingsconfirmedthenotionofMataetal.(2011)thatolderadultsdemonstrateriskaversionintheriskychoicecontextinwhichrisk-seekingwouldbeamoreeffectivestrategy. Thefactthatincidentalfailurescauseinitialincreasesinprecautionsimilarlyamongyoungerandolderadultsmaybebecauseitisafairlycommonlifeexperience,especiallyinthecontextofconsumerbehavior.Forinstance,abatchoflightbulbsofagivenbrandmayeasilyoverheat.Asaresult,consumersmayreactbyautomaticallyusingcautionwhenpurchasingproductsofthatbrand,buttheymaygraduallyandslowlychangewithmorepositiveexperiences.Itisinlinewithanumberofstudies(Queenetal.,2012;Hessetal.,2013),whichdemonstratethatagedifferencesdisappearinthedomainofconsumerbehaviorregardingwell-knownproducts.Itisthereforeexplainedbypossessinglifeexperiencesandbywell-preservedexperientialprocessesamongolderadults.Similarly,incidentalsuccessesareusuallyfollowedbytemporaryincreasestowardmoreriskybehaviors.Forexample,inarecentlypublishedstudyconductedinnaturalsettings,itwasfoundthatafterabasketballteam’svictory,residentsofthetownpurchasedsubstantiallymorelotterytickets(Ottoetal.,2016). TheMediatingRolesofProcessingSpeedandNeedforCognitiveClosure Furtheranalysesshowedthatprocessingspeed(asmeasuredbytheDSST)mediatesage-relateddifferencesinthebasicmeasuresoftheBART:theadjustednumberofpumps,theearnedpoints,andthenumberofexplosions.Therelationshipbetweenageandexplosionswasalsomediated(double-unrelatedmediators)bytheneedforclosuremeasure(NFCC_2).Thesefindingsconfirmedtheresultsofpreviousresearch(Henningeretal.,2010),whichusedadifferentmeasureofprocessingspeed.Additionally,ouranalysesidentifiedfullmediationsintermsofBaronandKenny’s(1986)classicalmediationmodel:thedirectandstrongeffectofageonBARTmeasuresdisappearedwhenprocessingspeed(andincaseofexplosions,alsotheneedforclosure)wasincludedinthemediationmodel.Onamoregenerallevel,thesefindingsareconsistentwiththegeneralroleofprocessingspeed(Salthouse,1996,2001)asthemediatoroftherelationshipbetweenageandperformanceofcomplexcognitivetasks.Morespecifically,thesefindingsagreewiththeresultsdescribedinmanyrecentstudies(e.g.,Finucaneetal.,2005;Mataetal.,2007;Freyetal.,2015)–thatprocessingspeedandsomeothercognitivevariables(e.g.,workingmemory)areimportantmediatorsofage-relateddifferencesincomplexdecision-makingtasks. Theconstructoftheneedforcognitiveclosure,whichincreaseswithage,alsodeservesmoreapplicationsinthedomainofagingandriskydecision-making.Researchersemphasizeitsimportanceateverystageoflife(Roetsetal.,2015);however,itappearstobeofspecialimportanceinolderage.IndividualswithhighNFCCareintolerantofconfusionanduncertainty,andthereforeareinclinedtomakerapiddecisions.Ontheotherhand,low-NFCCindividualsaremotivatedtoanalyzesituationsinasystematicmanner,consideralternativeoptions,andmakecomplexdecisions(KruglanskiandWebster,1996).MostpreviousresearchhasdemonstratedthatNFCCisrelatedtoasimplifiedcognitiveprocessinvolvedindecision-making,alimitedinformationsearch,andapreferenceforclearandunambiguousjudgment.Moreimportantly,ithasshownthathigh-NFCCindividualsarecharacterizedbyfewercognitiveresourcesthatcanbeallocatedtopresentactivities(Kossowska,2007;Kossowskaetal.,2010).Thus,inourrecentresearch(Czarneketal.,2015),wefoundthatagingdecreasedprocessingspeedandcognitiveflexibilityasmeasuredbytheTrailMakingTestandincreasedNFCC(thesametwoNFCCsubscalesusedhere)andthatbothfactorsmediatetherelationshipbetweenageanddrawingstereotypicalinferencesaboutout-groups.Inthiscurrentstudy,weagainfoundthedouble-mediationofacognitivefactor(mentalspeed)andamotivationfactor(needforcognitiveclosure)oftherelationshipbetweenageandthenumberofexplosionsexperiencedduringtheBART.Furtherresearchisneededtoelucidatetherolesofbothcognitiveandmotivationalmechanismsinage-relateddifferencesinriskychoices. TheRoleofInitialRiskPerceptionintheBART In-depthanalysesprovidedmoreinformationonparticipants’behaviorsconnectedwiththeinfluenceofincidentalfailureexperiences(threeunluckytrials)ondecision-makingunderriskintheBART.Aconsistentpatternofresultswasacquired,suggestingthatstudyparticipantspumptheballoonfarmoreboldlyafterasafebeginningthanafterexperiencingincidentalfailure.However,thisinfluencegraduallydisappearsasexperienceisgained,andtheeffectofpreviouslyexperiencedfailurebecomesweaker.Generally,thispatternofresultsispartiallyconsistentwiththepredictionsofcognitivemodelingstudies(Cavanaghetal.,2012;Rolisonetal.,2012;Seamanetal.,2015)thatinitialperceptionsofhighriskmightimpairBARTperformance.Thenewandintriguingevidenceisthatsuchmanipulationofincidentalfailureisverystrongandlastsacrosstheentireseries.Theadditionaladvantageofthisstudywasthatthestrengthofthiseffectwasconfirmedbyexperimentalmanipulation. Thispatternofresultsdoesnotdifferstatisticallyinbothagegroups,supportingtheexistinghypothesesthatolderadultshavetheabilitytoadapttheirbehaviortochangingconditions(Queenetal.,2012).Olderwomenwhoweresubjectedtoincidentalfailuresatthebeginningwerenotonlyabletoadjusttheirballoonpumpingstrategiestofurthertaskconditionsequallyefficientlyasyoungerwomen,butalsostabilizedthenumberofpumpsattheveryendtolevelsequalingthoseofparticipantssubjectedtoluckybeginnings.Maintainingsuchflexibilityindecision-makingisundoubtedlyanexampleofamechanismcompensatingforsomecognitivelimitations. LimitationsandFutureResearch Extendedreplicationsandlongitudinalparadigmsareneededtoestablishwhetherthedouble-mediationmodels(includingbothcognitiveandmotivationalvariables)arereliablefortheBARTandwhethertheycouldbeextendedtootherriskydecisionparadigms,suchastheIGT,whichshowedoppositetendenciesinaging(cf.Mataetal.,2011).Also,itwouldbeimportanttodeterminewhetherexperimentalmanipulationwithincidentalfailures(orsuccesses)willreplicatebothverystrongeffectsandgradualadaptationsinotherriskychoiceparadigmsandtoconfirmthepreservedabilityofolderadultstoefficientlycopewithsuchincidentalevents.Moreover,itwouldbeusefultoincludeaffectmeasureofthePANASXbothatthebeginningandattheendofthestudytocheckforpossibleinfluencesoftheBARTperformanceontheaffectiveexperienceofstudyparticipantsinfutureresearchoratleasttoincludeitattheverybeginningofthestudytoavoidtheinfluenceofBARTperformanceonaffect. LindenbergerandPötter(1998)underlinedthelimitationsofregressionmediatingmodelsinprovidingareliableaccountofvarianceexplainedbecauseofexistinginter-correlationsbetweenpredictorvariables.Theprimaryproblemisthatcorrelationsbetweentwovariablesthatareeachcorrelatedwithagemayresultinaspuriousrelationshipbetweenthetwovariablesthatdoesnotreflectatruerelationship.However,therearesomecounterargumentsagainstthepossibilityofsuchspuriousrelationshipsinouranalyses,showingthattheeffectsofageonBARTmeasuresaremediatedbymentalspeed(operationalizedbytheDSST).First,thezero-ordercorrelationsbetweenmentalspeedandBART(seeTable4)andrelationshipsbetweenmentalspeedandBARTinthemediationmodels(seeFigure3)preservedtheirsigns,values,andsignificancelevels.AsdemonstratedbyLindenbergerandPötter(1998),incaseofspuriouscorrelations,zero-ordercorrelationsbetweenmediatoranddependentvariablemayshowcompletelydifferentpatternsthanappropriaterelationshipswithinmediationmodels.Second,thezero-ordercorrelationsbetweenmentalspeedandBARTmeasureswerecalculatedseparatelyforeachagegroup.Fortheoldergroup,thesecorrelationsweresubstantialandsignificant(atleastp<0.05)withtheaveragenumberofpumps(r=0.35),thenumberofpoints(r=0.29),andthenumberofexplosions(r=0.30).However,thesecorrelationswerenotsignificantamongtheyoungergroup.Thesepatternsofcorrelationsareconsistentwiththeoreticalargumentsthatlargerproblemsofolderadults(comparedtoyoungeradults)inBARTperformanceareassociatedwithloweredmentalspeed.Generally,mediationanalysesusingextremeagegroupsshouldbetreatedwithcaution.However,LindenbergerandPötter(1998)advisedthereisnothingfundamentallywrongwithusingmediationalanalysestoshowdirectandindirecteffectsofagegroupsonpsychologicalprocessesiftherearesoundtheoreticalargumentsinfavorofsuchapathorstructuralmodels. Infuturestudies,itwouldbeimportanttoexamineothercognitivecomponents(i.e.,notonlyprocessingspeed,butalsoworkingmemoryspans;Engleetal.,1999)andothermotivationalcomponentsaspotentialmediatorsoftherelationshipbetweenagingandriskydecision-making.ExamplesarethePersonalNeedforStructureandNeedforCognition(seeNeubergandNewsom,1993;Cacioppoetal.,1996)andcardiovascularmeasuresofmotivation(seeHessandEnnis,2014). Itisimportant,infurtherstudies,tocollectreactiontimes.Theabsenceorpresenceofanyage-relateddifferencesorinteractionsmaybeconfoundedbydifferentstrategies.Forexample,ifolderadultstakemuchmoretimethanyoungeradultstocompletethetasks,thiswouldleadtodifferentinterpretationthanifolderadultscarriedoutthetasksveryquickly. Theobviouslimitationsofthisresearcharethecross-sectionalcharacterofitsdesignandlackofcomparablemaleparticipants.Insomeearlierexperimentsonriskpreferences,researchersprovedthatinaccordancewithcommonstereotypesmenindeeddoprefermoreriskystrategies(includingthedeterminationofneurologicalunderpinningsofthesepreferences,e.g.,Bollaetal.,2004);whileinotherstudies(includingthosewhichuseneuroimagingtechniques),nosuchdifferenceswerefound(Galvanetal.,2007).d’AcremontandVanderLinden(2006)showedthatdifferencesinriskpreferencescanbeexplainedbythechoiceofaspecificexperimentaltask,whichseemstoconfirmthethesisthatthesedifferencesresultmorefromthecontextthanbiologicalbases(Cauffmanetal.,2010).InearlierresearchusingtheBARTprocedure,greaterriskaversionwasfoundamongwomen(Lejuezetal.,2002);however,thisdidnothaveadirectimpactontheeffectivenessofdecisionsthatweremade.Moreover,neuronaldifferencesinthisrespectwerealsoreported(Cazzelletal.,2012),whichmaybetheresultofdifferentratesofdevelopmentofbrainstructuresinthesexes.Forthesereasons,wechosetofocusexclusivelyonwomen.Ononehand,thiscouldbeconsideredalimitation,butontheotherhand,iteliminatespossibledistortionsintheresultsbyunspecifiedgenderdifferences. Conclusion Inthisstudyweappliedthedual-processperspectiveofageeffectsondecisionmaking.Accordingtothistheoreticalperspectivetheinfluenceofagingondecisionmakingismoderatedbythetypeofprocessingdemands(deliberativevs.experiential).InlinewithpredictionsolderparticipantswerefoundtoexhibitsignificantlyhigherriskaversionandlowerBARTperformance(demandingdeliberativeprocesses)comparedtotheiryoungercounterparts,addingcompellingresearchevidencetothescarcenumberofstudiesofageeffectsonBART.Whatisimportantandnewinthedomainofagingandriskydecisionmaking,isthattheeffectofageonsomeBARTmeasureswasmediatednotonlybycognitive(processingspeed)butalsomotivational(needforcognitiveclosure)mechanisms.Wealsodiscovered,usingoriginalexperimentalmanipulation,thatolderandyoungeradultsshowsimilarpatternofadaptationstotheinitialfailure(vs.success),whichismostprobablyduetopreservedefficiencyofexperientialprocessesinolderage. Theagingpopulationfacesnewchallengesthatoftenrequiremakingimportantriskydecisionsinunfamiliarcircumstances.Thisstudyprovidesnewknowledgeaboutmechanismsthatallowolderadultstobuild,inadifferent(notnecessarilyworse)way,theirownstrategyforfunctioninginriskanduncertaintyconditions.Itturnsoutthatinsomecases,forinstancewhenexperiencingincidentalfailure,olderadultsperformaswellasyoungeradults,whichcallsintoquestionmanyexistingstereotypes.Abetterunderstandingofthemutualrolesofcognitive,motivational,andaffectivemechanisms(see:Petersetal.,2011;Verhaeghenetal.,2012)mayleadtogreaterawarenessamongolderadultsoftheiradvantagesaswellasdisadvantagesandtoformationofnewtrainingprogramspreventingimperfectdecision-makinginlateadulthood. AuthorContributions MK:designoftheresearch,dataacquisition,basicdataanalysisandinterpretation,articledrafting,finalapprovaloftheversiontobepublished,agreementtobeaccountableforallaspectsofthework;KR:designoftheresearch,dataacquisition,articledrafting,finalapprovaloftheversiontobepublished,agreementtobeaccountableforallaspectsofthework;GS:supervisionoftheproject,designoftheresearch,advanceddataanalysisandinterpretation,revisingthework,finalapprovaloftheversiontobepublished,agreementtobeaccountableforallaspectsofthework. Funding ThisworkwassupportedbytheNationalScienceCentre,Poland,underGrant2015/17/B/HS6/04185,awardedtoGS. ConflictofInterestStatement Theauthorsdeclarethattheresearchwasconductedintheabsenceofanycommercialorfinancialrelationshipsthatcouldbeconstruedasapotentialconflictofinterest. Acknowledgment TheauthorswouldliketothankMalgorzataKowalandMarcinDomaszewskiforassistancewithdatacollection. References Aklin,W.M.,Lejuez,C.W.,Zvolensky,M.J.,Kahler,C.W.,andGwadz,M.(2005).Evaluationofbehavioralmeasuresofrisktakingpropensitywithinnercityadolescents.Behav.Res.Ther43,215–228.doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.12.007 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Baltes,P.B.,andBaltes,M.M.(1990).“Psychologicalperspectivesonsuccessfulaging:themodelofselectiveoptimizationwithcompensation,”inSuccessfulAging:PerspectivesfromtheBehavioralSciences,edsP.B.BaltesandM.M.Baltes(NewYork,NY:CambridgeUniversityPress),1–34. PubMedAbstract|GoogleScholar Bargh,J.A.(1994).“TheFourHorsemenofautomaticity:awareness,efficiency,intention,andcontrolinsocialcognition,”inHandbookofSocialCognition,2ndEdn,edsR.S.WyerJr.andT.K.Srull(Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum),1–40. GoogleScholar Baron,R.M.,andKenny,D.A.(1986).Themoderator–mediatorvariabledistinctioninsocialpsychologicalresearch:conceptual,strategic,andstatisticalconsiderations.J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.51,1173–1182.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Bechara,A.,Damasio,A.R.,Damasio,H.,andAnderson,S.W.(1994).Insensitivitytofutureconsequencesfollowingdamagetohumanprefrontalcortex.Cognition50,7–15.doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Blanchard-Fields,F.,Hertzog,C.,Stein,R.,andPak,R.(2001).Beyondastereotypedviewofolderadults’traditionalfamilyvalues.Psychol.Aging16,483–496.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.16.3.483 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Bolla,K.I.,Eldreth,D.A.,Matochik,J.A.,andCadet,J.L.(2004).Sex-relateddifferencesinagamblingtaskanditsneurologicalcorrelates.Cereb.Cortex14,1226–1232.doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh083 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Bornovalova,M.A.,Cashman-Rolls,A.,O’Donnell,J.M.,Ettinger,K.,Richards,J.B.,DeWit,H.,etal.(2009).Risktakingdifferencesonabehavioraltaskasafunctionofpotentialreward/lossmagnitudeandindividualdifferencesinimpulsivityandsensationseeking.Pharmacol.Biochem.Behav.93,258–262.doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2008.10.023 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Brown,K.(2002).ImpactofStockMarketDeclineon50–70YearOldInvestors.Washington,DC:AARP,KnowledgeManagement. GoogleScholar BruinedeBruin,W.,Strough,J.,andParker,A.M.(2014).Gettingolderisn’tallthatbad:betterdecisionsandcopingwhenfacingsunkcosts.Psychol.Aging29,642–647.doi:10.1037/a0036308 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Cacioppo,J.T.,Petty,R.E.,Feinstein,J.A.,andJarvis,W.B.G.(1996).Dispositionaldifferencesincognitivemotivation:thelifeandtimesofindividualsvaryinginneedforcognition.Psychol.Bull.119,197–253.doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.197 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Carstensen,L.L.(2006).Theinfluenceofasenseoftimeonhumandevelopment.Science312,1913–1915.doi:10.1126/science.1127488 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Carstensen,L.L.,Isaacowitz,D.M.,andCharles,S.T.(1999).Takingtimeseriously:atheoryofsocioemotionalselectivity.Am.Psychol.54,165–181.doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Cauffman,E.,Shulman,E.P.,Steinberg,L.,Claus,E.,Banich,M.T.,Graham,S.,etal.(2010).AgedifferencesinaffectivedecisionmakingasindexedbyperformanceontheIowaGamblingTask.Dev.Psychol.46,193–207.doi:10.1037/a0016128 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Cavanagh,J.F.,Neville,D.,Cohen,M.X.,VandeVijver,I.,Harsay,H.,Watson,P.,etal.(2012).Individualdifferencesinriskydecision-makingamongseniorsreflectincreasedrewardsensitivity.Front.Neurosci.6:111.doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00111 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Cazzell,M.,Li,L.,Lin,Z.J.,Patel,S.J.,andLiu,H.(2012).Comparisonofneuralcorrelatesofriskdecisionmakingbetweengenders:anexploratoryfNIRSstudyoftheBalloonAnalogueRiskTask(BART).Neuroimage62,1896–1911.doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.030 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Charles,S.T.,andCarstensen,L.L.(2010).Socialandemotionalaging.Annu.Rev.Psychol.61,383–409.doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100448 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Chaubey,N.P.(1974).Effectofageonexpectancyofsuccessandonrisk-takingbehavior.J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.29,774–778.doi:10.1037/h0036178 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Chou,K.-L.,Lee,T.M.C.,andHo,A.H.Y.(2007).Doesmoodstatechangerisktakingtendencyinolderadults?Psychol.Aging22,310–318.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.2.310 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Cornelis,I.,VanHiel,A.,Roets,A.,andKossowska,M.(2009).Agedifferencesinconservatism:evidenceonthemediatingeffectsofpersonalityandcognitivestyle.J.Pers.77,51–88.doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00538.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Czarnek,G.,Kossowska,M.,andSedek,G.(2015).Theinfluenceofagingonoutgroupstereotypes:themediatingroleofcognitiveandmotivationalfacetsofdeficientflexibility.Exp.AgingRes.41,303–324.doi:10.1080/0361073X.2015.1021647 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar d’Acremont,M.,andVanderLinden,M.(2006).Genderdifferencesintwodecision-makingtasksinacommunitysampleofadolescents.Int.J.Behav.Dev.30,352–358.doi:10.1177/0165025406066740 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Deakin,J.,Aitken,M.,Robbins,T.,andSahakian,B.J.(2004).Risktakingduringdecision-makinginnormalvolunteerschangeswithage.J.Int.Neuropsychol.Soc.10,590–598.doi:10.1017/S1355617704104104 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Denburg,N.L.,Weller,J.A.,Yamada,T.H.,Shivapour,D.M.,Kaup,A.R.,LaLoggia,A.,etal.(2009).Poordecisionmakingamongolderadultsisrelatedtoelevatedlevelsofneuroticism.Ann.Behav.Med.37,164–172.doi:10.1007/s12160-009-9094-7 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Dror,I.E.,Katona,M.,andMungur,K.(1998).Agedifferencesindecisionmaking:totakeariskornot?Gerontology44,67–71.doi:10.1159/000021986 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Engle,R.W.,Sedek,G.,vonHecker,U.,andMcIntosh,D.N.(eds).(2005).CognitiveLimitationsinAgingandPsychopathology.NewYork,NY:CambridgeUniversityPress. GoogleScholar Engle,R.W.,Tuholski,S.W.,Laughlin,J.E.,andConway,A.R.A.(1999).Workingmemory,short-termmemory,andgeneralfluidintelligence:alatent-variableapproach.J.Exp.Psychol.Gen.128,309–331.doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Epstein,S.(1994).Integrationofthecognitiveandthepsychodynamicunconscious.Am.Psychol.49,709–724.doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.709 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Fajkowska,M.,andMarszał-Wiśniewska,M.(2009).WłaściwościpsychometryczneSkaliPozytywnegoiNegatywnegoAfektu–WersjaRozszerzona(PANAS-X).Wstkepnewynikibadańwpolskiejpróbie.[PsychometricpropertiesofthePositiveandNegativeAffectSchedule-ExpandedForm(PANAS-X).ThestudyonaPolishsample].Prz.Psychol.52,355–388. GoogleScholar Finucane,M.L.,Mertz,C.K.,Slovic,P.,andSchmidt,E.S.(2005).Taskcomplexityandolderadults’decision-makingcompetence.Psychol.Aging20,71–84.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.71 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Frederick,S.(2005).Cognitivereflectionanddecisionmaking.J.Econ.Perspect.19,25–42.doi:10.1257/089533005775196732 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Frey,R.,Mata,R.,andHertwig,R.(2015).Theroleofcognitiveabilitiesindecisionsfromexperience:agedifferencesemergeasafunctionofchoicesetsize.Cognition142,60–80.doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.004 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Galvan,A.,Hare,T.,Voss,H.,Glover,G.,andCasey,B.J.(2007).Risk-takingandtheadolescentbrain:whoisatrisk?Dev.Sci.10,F8–F14.doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00579.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hayes,A.F.(2013).IntroductiontoMediation,Moderation,andConditionalProcessAnalysis:ARegression-BasedApproach.NewYork,NY:GuilfordPress. GoogleScholar Hayes,A.F.,andScharkow,M.(2013).Therelativetrustworthinessofinferentialtestsoftheindirecteffectinstatisticalmediationanalysis:doesmethodreallymatter?Psychol.Sci.24,1918–1927.doi:10.1177/0956797613480187 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Henninger,D.E.,Madden,D.J.,andHuettel,S.A.(2010).Processingspeedandmemorymediateage-relateddifferencesindecisionmaking.Psychol.Aging25,262–270.doi:10.1037/a0019096 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hess,T.M.(2000).“Aging-relatedconstraintsandadaptiationinsocialinformationprocessing,”inGenerativeMentalProcessesandCognitiveResources:IntegrativeResearchonAdaptationandControl,edsU.VonHecker,S.Dutke,andG.Sedek(Dordrecht:KluwerAcademicPublishers),129–156. GoogleScholar Hess,T.M.(2014).Selectiveengagementofcognitiveresources:motivationalinfluencesonolderadults’cognitivefunctioning.Perspect.Psychol.Sci.9,388–407.doi:10.1177/1745691614527465 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hess,T.M.(2015).“Aprospecttheory-basedevaluationofdual-processinfluencesonaginganddecisionmaking:supportforacontextualperspective,”inAgingandDecisionMaking:EmpiricalandAppliedPerspectives,edsT.M.Hess,J.Strough,andC.E.Loeckenhoff(NewYork,NY:Elsevier),189–212. GoogleScholar Hess,T.M.,andEnnis,G.E.(2014).Assessmentofadultagedifferencesintaskengagement:theutilityofsystolicbloodpressure.Motiv.Emot.38,844–854.doi:10.1007/s11031-014-9433-2 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hess,T.M.,Queen,T.L.,andEnnis,G.E.(2013).Ageandself-relevanceeffectsoninformationsearchduringdecisionmaking.J.Gerontol.BPsychol.68,703–711.doi:10.1093/geronb/gbs108 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hess,T.M.,Queen,T.L.,andPatterson,T.(2012).Todeliberateornottodeliberate:interactionsbetweenage,taskcharacteristics,andcognitiveactivityondecisionmaking.J.Behav.Decis.Mak.25,29–40.doi:10.1002/bdm.711 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hopko,D.R.,Lejuez,C.W.,Daughters,S.B.,Aklin,W.M.,Osborne,A.,Simmons,B.L.,etal.(2006).ConstructvalidityoftheBalloonAnalogueRiskTask(BART):relationshipwithMDMAusebyinner-citydrugusersinresidentialtreatment.J.Psychopathol.Behav.Assess.28,95–101.doi:10.1007/s10862-006-7487-5 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hunt,M.K.,Hopko,D.R.,Bare,R.,Lejuez,C.W.,andRobinson,E.V.(2005).ConstructvalidityoftheBalloonAnalogRiskTask(BART):associationswithpsychopathyandimpulsivity.Assessment12,416–428.doi:10.1177/1073191105278740 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kahneman,D.(2003).Aperspectiveonjudgmentandchoice:mappingboundedrationality.Am.Psychol.58,697–720.doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kossowska,M.(2007).Motivationtowardclosureandcognitiveprocesses:anindividualdifferencesapproach.Pers.Indiv.Diff.43,2149–2158.doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.027 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kossowska,M.,Hanusz,K.,andTrejtowicz,M.(2012a).SkróconawersjaSkaliPotrzebyPoznawczegoDomknikecia.Dobórpozycjiiwalidacjaskali.[ShortversionoftheNeedforCognitiveClosureScale:itemsselectionandscalevalidation].Psychol.Społ.7,89–99. Kossowska,M.,Jaśko,K.,Bar-Tal,Y.,andSzastok,M.(2012b).Therelationshipbetweenneedforclosureandmemoryforschema-relatedinformationamongyoungerandolderadults.Neuropsychol.Dev.Cogn.BAgingNeuropsychol.Cogn.19,283–300.doi:10.1080/13825585.2011.632617 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kossowska,M.,Orehek,E.,andKruglanski,A.W.(2010).“Motivationtowardsclosureandcognitiveresources:anindividualdifferencesapproach,”inHandbookofIndividualDifferencesinCognition:Attention,MemoryandExecutiveControl,edsA.Gruszka,G.Mathews,andB.Szymura(NewYork,NY:SpringerScience+BusinessMedia),369–382. GoogleScholar Kovalchik,S.,Camerer,C.F.,Grether,D.M.,Plott,C.R.,andAllman,J.M.(2005).Aginganddecisionmaking:acomparisonbetweenneurologicallyhealthyelderlyandyoungindividuals.J.Econ.Behav.Organ.58,79–94.doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2003.12.001 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kruglanski,A.,andWebster,D.(1996).Motivatedclosingofthemind:“Seizing”and“freezing”.Psychol.Rev.2,263–283.doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.263 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kruglanski,A.W.(2004).ThePsychologyofClosedMindedness.NewYork,NY:PsychologyPress. GoogleScholar Lauriola,M.,Panno,A.,Levin,I.P.,andLejuez,C.W.(2014).Individualdifferencesinriskydecisionmaking:ameta-analysisofsensationseekingandimpulsivitywiththeBalloonAnalogueRiskTask.J.Behav.Decis.Mak.27,20–36.doi:10.1002/bdm.1784 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Lejuez,C.W.,Aklin,W.M.,Jones,H.A.,Richards,J.B.,Strong,D.R.,Kahler,C.W.,etal.(2003).TheBalloonAnalogueRiskTask(BART)differentiatessmokersandnonsmokers.Exp.Clin.Psychopharmacol.11,26–33.doi:10.1037/1064-1297.11.1.26 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Lejuez,C.W.,Read,J.P.,Kahler,C.W.,Richards,J.B.,Ramsey,S.E.,Stuart,G.L.,etal.(2002).Evaluationofabehavioralmeasureofrisktaking:theBalloonAnalogueRiskTask(BART).J.Exp.Psychol.Appl.8,75–84.doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Lindenberger,U.,andPötter,U.(1998).Thecomplexnatureofuniqueandsharedeffectsinhierarchicallinearregression:implicationsfordevelopmentalpsychology.Psychol.Methods3,218–230.doi:10.1037/1082-989X.3.2.218 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar MacPherson,S.E.,Phillips,L.H.,andDellaSala,S.(2002).Age,executivefunctionandsocialdecisionmaking:adorsolateralprefrontaltheoryofcognitiveaging.Psychol.Aging17,598–609.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.598 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Mata,R.,Josef,A.K.,Samanez-Larkin,G.R.,andHertwig,R.(2011).Agedifferencesinriskychoice:ameta-analysis.Ann.N.Y.Acad.Sci.1235,18–29.doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06200.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Mata,R.,Schooler,L.J.,andRieskamp,J.(2007).Theagingdecisionmaker:cognitiveagingandtheadaptiveselectionofdecisionstrategies.Psychol.Aging22,796–810.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.796 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Neuberg,S.L.,andNewsom,J.(1993).Individualdifferencesinchronicmotivationtosimplify:personalneedforstructureandsocial-cognitiveprocessing.J.Pers.Soc.Psychol.65,113–131.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.113 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Otto,A.R.,Fleming,S.M.,andGlimcher,P.W.(2016).Unexpectedbutincidentalpositiveoutcomespredictreal-worldgambling.Psychol.Sci.27,299–311.doi:10.1177/0956797615618366 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Peters,E.,Dieckmann,N.E.,andWeller,J.(2011).“Agedifferencesincomplexdecisionmaking,”inHandbookofthePsychologyofAging,7thEdn,edsK.W.Schaie,S.L.Willis,K.W.Schaie,andS.L.Willis(SanDiego,CA:Elsevier),133–151. GoogleScholar Peters,E.,Hess,T.M.,Västfjäll,D.,andAuman,C.(2007).Adultagedifferencesindualinformationprocesses:implicationsfortheroleofaffectiveanddeliberativeprocessesinolderadults’decisionmaking.Perspect.Psychol.Sci.2,1–23.doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00025.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Queen,T.L.,andHess,T.M.(2010).Agedifferencesintheeffectsofconsciousandunconsciousthoughtindecisionmaking.Psychol.Aging25,251–261.doi:10.1037/a0018856 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Queen,T.L.,Hess,T.M.,Ennis,G.E.,Dowd,K.,andGrühn,D.(2012).Informationsearchanddecisionmaking:effectsofageandcomplexityonstrategyuse.Psychol.Aging27,817–824.doi:10.1037/a0028744 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Roets,A.,Kruglanski,A.W.,Kossowska,M.,Pierro,A.,andHong,Y.-Y.(2015).“Themotivatedgatekeeperofourminds,”inAdvancesinExperimentalSocialPsychology,Vol.52,edsJ.M.OlsonandM.P.Zanna(London:Elsevier),221–283. Rolison,J.J.,Hanoch,Y.,andWood,S.(2012).Riskydecisionmakinginyoungerandolderadults:theroleoflearning.Psychol.Aging27,129–140.doi:10.1037/a0024689 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Salthouse,T.(2006).Mentalexerciseandmentalaging.Perspect.Psychol.Sci.1,68–87.doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00005.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Salthouse,T.(2012).Consequencesofage-relatedcognitivedecline.Ann.Rev.Psychol.63,201–226.doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100328 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Salthouse,T.A.(1996).Theprocessing-speedtheoryofadultagedifferencesincognition.Psychol.Rev.103,403–428.doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Salthouse,T.A.(2001).Structuralmodelsoftherelationsbetweenageandmeasuresofcognitivefunctioning.Intelligence29,93–115.doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.01.001 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Samanez-Larkin,G.R.,Wagner,A.D.,andKnutson,B.(2011).Expectedvalueinformationimprovesfinancialrisktakingacrosstheadultlifespan.Soc.Cogn.Affect.Neurosci.6,207–217.doi:10.1093/scan/nsq043 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Seaman,K.L.,Stillman,C.M.,Howard,D.V.,andHoward,J.H.Jr.(2015).Riskydecision-makingisassociatedwithresidentialchoiceinhealthyolderadults.Front.Psychol.6:1192.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01192 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Sedek,G.,Kossowska,M.,andRydzewska,K.(2014).Theimportanceofadultlife-spanperspectiveinexplainingvariationsinpoliticalideology.Behav.BrainSci.37,329–330.doi:10.1017/S0140525X13002732 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Sedek,G.,Verhaeghen,P.,andMartin,M.(eds)(2013).SocialandMotivationalCompensatoryMechanismsofAge-relatedCognitiveDecline.SpecialIssueofAging,Neuropsychology,andCognition.Hove:PsychologyPress. Sedek,G.,andvonHecker,U.(2004).Effectsofsubclinicaldepressionandagingongenerativereasoningaboutlinearorders:sameordifferentprocessinglimitations?J.Exp.Psychol.Gen.133,237–260.doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.237 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar StatisticBrainResearchInstitute(2015).CEOStatistics.Availableat:http://www.statisticbrain.com/ceo-statistics/ Thornton,W.J.L.,andDumke,H.(2005).Agedifferencesineverydayproblemsolvinganddecisionmakingeffectiveness:ameta-analyticreview.Psychol.Aging20,85–99.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.85 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Verhaeghen,P.,Martin,M.,andSedek,G.(2012).Reconnectingcognitioninthelabandcognitioninreallife:theroleofcompensatorysocialandmotivationalfactorsinexplaininghowcognitionagesinthewild.Neuropsychol.Dev.Cogn.BAgingNeuropsychol.Cogn.19,1–12.doi:10.1080/13825585.2011.645009 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar vonHelversen,B.,andMata,R.(2012).Losingadimewithasatisfiedmind:positiveaffectpredictslesssearchinsequentialdecisionmaking.Psychol.Aging4,825–839.doi:10.1037/a0027845 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Watson,D.,andClark,L.A.(1994).ThePANAS-X:ManualforthePositiveandNegativeAffectSchedule-ExpandedForm.Availableat:http://ir.uiowa.edu/ GoogleScholar Wechsler,D.(1981).WAIS-RManual:WechslerAdultIntelligenceScale-Revised.NewYork,NY:ThePsychologicalCorporation. GoogleScholar Wood,S.,Busemeyer,J.,Koling,A.,Cox,C.R.,andDavis,H.(2005).Olderadultsasadaptivedecisionmakers:evidencefromtheIowaGamblingTask.Psychol.Aging20,220–225.doi:10.1037/0882-7974.20.2.220 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Keywords:aging,risktaking,BART,initialriskperception,processingspeed,needforcognitiveclosure Citation:KoscielniakM,RydzewskaKandSedekG(2016)EffectsofAgeandInitialRiskPerceptiononBalloonAnalogRiskTask:TheMediatingRoleofProcessingSpeedandNeedforCognitiveClosure.Front.Psychol.7:659.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00659 Received:26December2015;Accepted:20April2016;Published:06May2016. Editedby: EmmaV.Ward,MiddlesexUniversity,UK Reviewedby: ThomasHess,NorthCarolinaStateUniversity,USA LoreenMamerow,UniversityofBasel,Switzerland Copyright©2016Koscielniak,RydzewskaandSedek.Thisisanopen-accessarticledistributedunderthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionLicense(CCBY).Theuse,distributionorreproductioninotherforumsispermitted,providedtheoriginalauthor(s)orlicensorarecreditedandthattheoriginalpublicationinthisjournaliscited,inaccordancewithacceptedacademicpractice.Nouse,distributionorreproductionispermittedwhichdoesnotcomplywiththeseterms. *Correspondence:GrzegorzSedek,[email protected] COMMENTARY ORIGINALARTICLE Peoplealsolookedat SuggestaResearchTopic>



請為這篇文章評分?