Feel the Time. Time Perception as a Function of Interoceptive ...
文章推薦指數: 80 %
Time perception is a fundamental element of human awareness. Our consciousness, our ability to perceive the world around us and, ultimately, ... Articles IrinaStrigo UniversityofCalifornia,SanFrancisco,UnitedStates J.MichaelHerrmann UniversityofEdinburgh,UnitedKingdom MarcWittmann InstituteforFrontierAreasofPsychologyandMentalHealth(IGPP),Germany Theeditorandreviewers'affiliationsarethelatestprovidedontheirLoopresearchprofilesandmaynotreflecttheirsituationatthetimeofreview. Abstract Introduction MaterialsandMethods Results Discussion AuthorContributions ConflictofInterestStatement Acknowledgments Footnotes References SuggestaResearchTopic> DownloadArticle DownloadPDF ReadCube EPUB XML(NLM) Supplementary Material Exportcitation EndNote ReferenceManager SimpleTEXTfile BibTex totalviews ViewArticleImpact SuggestaResearchTopic> SHAREON OpenSupplementalData ORIGINALRESEARCHarticle Front.Hum.Neurosci.,06March2018 |https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00074 FeeltheTime.TimePerceptionasaFunctionofInteroceptiveProcessing DanieleDiLernia1*,SilviaSerino1,2,GiovanniPezzulo3,ElisaPedroli2,PietroCipresso1,2andGiuseppeRiva1,2 1DepartmentofPsychology,UniversitàCattolicadelSacroCuore,Milan,Italy 2AppliedTechnologyforNeuro-PsychologyLab,IRCCSIstitutoAuxologicoItaliano,Milan,Italy 3InstituteofCognitiveSciencesandTechnologies,NationalResearchCouncil,Rome,Italy Thenatureoftimeisrootedinourbody.Constellationsofimpulsesarisingfromthefleshconstantlycreateourinteroceptiveperceptionand,inturn,theunfoldingoftheseperceptionsdefineshumanawarenessoftime.ThisstudyexploredtheconnectionbetweentimeperceptionandinteroceptionandproposestheInteroceptiveBuffersaturation(IBs)index.IBsevaluatessubjects’abilitytoprocesssalientstimulifromthebodybymeasuringsubjectivedistortionsofinteroceptivetimeperception,i.e.,theestimateddurationoftactileinteroceptivestimulations.Thirtyfemalehealthysubjectswererecruitedthroughconsecutivesamplingandassessedforcommonvariablesrelatedtointeroceptivealterations:depressivesymptoms(BeckDepressionInventory,BDI-II),eatingdisorders(EDI-3)risk,andanxietylevels(StateTraitAnxietyInventory,STAI).Interoceptivecardiacaccuracy(IAc)wasassessedaswell.Subjectsperformedverbaltimeestimationofinteroceptivestimuli(IBs)deliveredusingaspecificallydesignedinteroceptivetactilestimulator,aswellasverbaltimeestimationofvisualandauditorystimuli.ResultsshowedthatIBsindexpositivelycorrelatedwithIAc,andnegativelywithEDI-3DriveforThinness(DT)risksubscale.Moreover,IBsindexwaspositivelypredictedbyIAc,andnegativelypredictedbyDTandsomaticfactorsofdepression.Ourresultssuggestthatunderestimationsininteroceptivetimeperceptionareconnectedtodifferentpsychologicalconditionscharacterizedbyadiminishedprocessingofhighsaliencestimulifromthebody.Conversely,overestimationsofthedurationofinteroceptivestimuliappeartobefunctionofsubjects’abilitytocorrectlyperceivetheirownbodilyinformation.EvidencesupportedIBsindex,fosteringtheconceptofinteroceptivetreatmentsforclinicalpurposes. Introduction Timeperceptionisafundamentalelementofhumanawareness.Ourconsciousness,ourabilitytoperceivetheworldaroundusand,ultimately,ourverysenseofselfareshapeduponourperceptionoftimeinloopconnectingmemoriesofthepast,presentsensationsandexpectationsaboutthefuture.Yet,thewayweperceivetimeiswidelydebated. ScalarExpectancyTheory(Gibbonetal.,1984)isoneofthemostacceptedframeworksoftimeperception(Church,1984;Treismanetal.,1990).AcoretenetofSETisaninternalclockwithapacemaker-accumulatorcomponent.Pulsesemittedbythepacemakerarestoredintheaccumulator,andtheamountofunitsstoredinafinitespaninfluencessamplefrequencyandourtimeperception.Ahighpulseratewillstoremoreunitsintheaccumulator,thereforeleadingtooverestimationinsubjectiveperception,whereasalowpulseratewillproduceoppositeeffects.RecentdevelopmentsofSETincludedmemoryanddecisionmakingcomponentsalongwiththepacemaker-accumulatorunit,providingamoreefficientneurocognitiveframeworkfortimeawareness(Gibbonetal.,1984;Luietal.,2011).Moreover,theAttentionalGateModel(ZakayandBlock,1996)introducedattentionasmediatorofthemodeswitchbetweenthepacemakerandtheaccumulator.Specifically,attentioncancontrolthemodeswitch(WeardenandPenton-Voak,1995;Droit-VoletandMeck,2007;Weardenetal.,2010;Ogdenetal.,2015a)insuchawaythat,iftheswitchisopen,someemittedpulsescanbelostthereforecontractingourperceptionoftime(BurleandCasini,2001;Wittmann,2009;Ogden,2013). Thepacemaker-accumulatorframeworkhasbeenconnectedtoanembodiedmodeloftimeperception(WittmannandvanWassenhove,2009;Wittmannetal.,2010)byseveralauthors.Inthisperspective,bodilystatesandemotionsrepresentcentralelementswhereashighphysiologicalarousal(GilandDroit-Volet,2012;Grecuccietal.,2014;Ogdenetal.,2015a;YooandLee,2015;Mionietal.,2016)canincreasethepulsefrequencyofthepacemaker,creatingoverestimationofsubjectivetimeperception(Wittmann,2009).Numerousotherauthorsalsosuggestedthatarousalandbodilysignalsaredeeplyconnectedwithsubjectivetimeawareness(Droit-VoletandGil,2009;GilandDroit-Volet,2012;Pollatosetal.,2014;Ogdenetal.,2015a,b),leadingtotheconclusionthatperceptionoftimeisintimatelyrootedinourbody. Anembodiedperspectiveoftimeisalsosupportedbyvariouscomputationalneuroscientificmodelsthathighlighttheimportanceofthebodyandofbodilymovements.EvidencefromTomassiniandMorrone(2016)suggestedthatsubjectiveperceptionoftimeisconnectedtothemotorcortexwhereassensory-motorconflictsintegrationcontributestosubjectivedistortionoftimeacrossdifferentmodalities.Inasimilarway,Orgsetal.(2013)linkedtheperceptionoftimetotheprocessingofhumanmovementsthatsubsidesvisualandmotorcorticalareas.Moreover,complementaryevidenceidentifiedadirectlinkbetweentemporalevaluationandvisuo-motorrepresentationofmotoractions,highlightingtheconnectionbetweentimeperceptionandbodymovements(Gavazzietal.,2013)alsoonrepresentativelevel.Perceptionoftimeappearsthereforeasafundamentalrequirementofseveralfunctionsrelatedtothebody.Tothisaccount,BuonomanoandLaje(2010)proposedtheconceptof“populationclocks”thatenvisionsperceptionoftimeasanemergingtraitofrecurrentlyconnectedneuralnetworksthatencodetime,andspecificallymotortime,intheactivitypatternsofapopulationofneurons(Buonomano,2014).Alongwithpreviousdiscussedevidence,theseconvergingdatasupportthereforeanembodiedperspectiveoftimeperceptioninhumans(KranjecandChatterjee,2010;Wittmann,2014)whichintegratesseveralsources,fromemotionstobodymovements,tocreateourawarenessoftime. FurthersupporttotheembodiedperspectiveoftimecomesfromCraig’srecentworkonthe“interoceptivematrix”anditsrelationstohumantimeawareness.Theinteroceptivematrixlocatedintheanteriorinsularcortex(AIC)receivesafferentinputsfromsmalldiametersensoryfibersthroughthelaminaIspinothalamocorticalpathway,whichcarriesfundamentalinformationfromallthetissuesofthebody(Craig,2002,2003,2009;Guetal.,2013;GuandFitzGerald,2014)creatinginteroceptiveperceptions.RecentstudiesshowedselectiveactivationoftheAICintimeperception(Raoetal.,2001;Coull,2004;LewisandMiall,2006;Liveseyetal.,2007)specificallywithintherangeofsub-secondstoseconds,confirmingAICasoneofthecoreconstituentsofhumanawarenessoftime. Acentralcomponentoftheinteroceptivematrixistheinteroceptivebuffer(Craig,2009)thatprocessesandcomparesinteroceptiveinformationwithpreviousandpaststatesofthebody,inordertopredictfutureconditionsoftheorganism(Friston,2009,2010;Fristonetal.,2011;Seth,2013;Ondobakaetal.,2017).Theseinteroceptivepredictionsservetooptimizethefunctioningoftheorganism,thuspromotingpredictionregulation—acontrol-theoreticprocessthathasbeencharacterizedintermsofallostasis(Sterling,2012)or,moreformally,asapredictionerror(orfree-energy)minimization(Sethetal.,2011;Seth,2013;Suzukietal.,2013). Craig(2009)proposedthattheinteroceptivebuffermayplayakeyroleintimeperceptionaswell.Thisisbecausethebufferhasafinitedimensionanditcanbeeasilyfilledupwithinteroceptiveinputs,alteringourperceptionoftime.Specifically,highrateofsalientstimulicansaturatethefinitedimensionofthebuffer,speedingupthesamplingfrequency,effectivelyslowing(Tseetal.,2004;CampbellandBryant,2007;vanWassenhoveetal.,2008;WittmannandPaulus,2008;Droit-Voletetal.,2011)theperceptionoftime,whichwillappearto“standstilltothesubjectiveobserver”(Craig,2009).Contrary,whentheinteroceptivebufferisnotfilledup“largeintervalsoftimeintheobjectiveworldcanappeartopassquickly”(Craig,2009). Nonetheless,somestudiesrevealedaparadoxintimeperception,whenhighratesofhighsaliencestimulicanproduceoppositeeffects(Droit-VoletandMeck,2007;Droit-VoletandGil,2009).Thesefindingsmaybeexplainedbyafunctionalprocessinglateralizationofdifferentinteroceptivevalences.Specifically,AICappearedtobeasymmetricallyactivatedindefiniteconditions,whereasparasympatheticinputsarepreferentiallyprocessedbythemidandleftinsula,whilesympatheticactivityisusuallyprocessedbytherightAIC(Craig,2009).AccordingtoCraig’semotionalasymmetryperspective,whenweexperienceadominantsympatheticarousal,stimuliprocessedintherightAICspeedupthesampleratefrequency,accumulatingpulsesintheinternalclock,leadingthereforetooverestimationofsubjectivetimeperception.Conversely,whenweareengagedinaparasympathetic(e.g.,affiliative)activation,stimuliarepreferentiallyprocessedbytheleftAIC,leadingtoasubjectiveunderestimationoftimeduetoalackofsympatheticactivity(Craig,2009). Thepresentstudystartedfromthehypothesisthattimeperceptionisintimatelyrelatedtothefunctioningoftheinteroceptivebuffer(Craig,2009).However,whilepreviousstudiesexploredhowbodilystatescanaltersubjectivetimeperception,ourstudyfocusedontheoppositepossibility:thatis,thepossibilitytoassessthedegreeofbodilyinputprocessingthroughdistortionsofinteroceptivetimeperception.ThekeyideaofInteroceptiveBuffersaturation(IBs)indexistoassessparasympatheticinteroceptivestimuli,whicharepreferentiallyprocessedbytheleftinsula(Gordonetal.,2013)fordistortionsinsubjectivetimeestimationduetothesympatheticworkloadofthecoactiverightinsula.Parasympatheticinteroceptivestimuliarereproduciblethroughaspecifickindoftouchcalledaffectivetouch(Olaussonetal.,2002,2016).ThiskindoftouchencompassesactivationofC-Tafferentsconnectedtotheinteroceptivesystem.Tothisgoal,IBsdesignusesacustomdeveloped“interoceptivestimulator”abletosendinteroceptivetactileinputs,consequentlymeasuringdistortioninsubjectivetimeestimationofthesestimuli.Theestimateddegreeoftimedistortioncanthereforeprovideinsightonsubject’sbodilyinformationprocessingduetotherelativeinterferenceofotherinteroceptiveinputsinthesubjectivetimeestimation. Tothebestofourknowledge,interoceptivebufferhasneverbeenoperationalizedneitherexperimentallyexplored,thereforeourresultscanprovidepowerfultheoreticalandclinicalinsightsregardingtherelationshipbetweenbodilystatesandinteroceptiveprocessing. Fromapracticalpointofview,therationalebehindtheIBsindexistoreverseengineertheconnectionbetweentheinteroceptivematrixandthesubjectiveperceptionoftime,feedinginteroceptivestimulithroughtheC-TafferentsinthesecondarytouchsystemconnectedtothelaminaIspinothalamocorticalpathway.UsingtheIBsmethodology,subjectiveoverestimationsoftimewouldsuggestadominanceofsympatheticarousal,whilesubjectiveunderestimationsoftimewouldsuggesttheoppositecondition. AccordingtotheemotionalasymmetryframeworkC-T(affective)touch(Olaussonetal.,2002;Ackerleyetal.,2014a,b;LiljencrantzandOlausson,2014)comprisesaparasympatheticactivationprimarilyprocessedbytheleftAIC(Gordonetal.,2013)thatmightleadtoanunderestimationoftimeperceptioninhealthysubjects(Craig,2009).Nonetheless,contextualinterferencescausedbysympatheticstimuli—suchashigharousingnegativeones—processedbytherightAICshouldinterferewiththisendogenoustimebasecreatingdistortionsinperceptiontowardsanoverestimationoftime. Numerousstudiessuggestedanasymmetriceffectofsympatheticandparasympatheticinputontimeperception.Indeed,timeperceptionofparasympatheticinteroceptiverelatedstimuliappearedunderestimatedinnormalconditionswithoutsympatheticdominantactivation(Ogdenetal.,2015b);conversely,inducedsympatheticstimuliareabletodirectlyaltertheinternaltimebaseline,paradoxicallyleadingtoamoreaccurateperception(Mellaetal.,2011)endorsingthenotionofanemotionaladvantageforhomeostaticregulation(Ogdenetal.,2015a)asalsoconfirmedbyotherauthors(Angrillietal.,1997;BuettiandLleras,2012;Droit-Voletetal.,2013;Pezzuloetal.,2018). Comprehendingandstudyingtheinteroceptivebufferhasparamountvalue.Correctaccesstointeroceptiveinformationiskeytoallostasisandadaptiveregulationoftheorganism,whereasdifferentconditionssuchasanorexianervosa(Pollatosetal.,2008)depression(Dunnetal.,2007)andchronicpain(DiLerniaetal.,2016b)appearedconnectedtoalterationsininteroceptiveprocessing.Althoughseveralotherindexesarecurrentlyavailabletoassessdifferentinteroceptivefactors(Garfinkeletal.,2015),IBsindexmayprovideanadvancedinstrumentwiththeabilitynotonlytoidentifyspecificalterationsbutalsothenatureandthedirectionoftheprocessesinvolvedinthesealterations.Commoninteroceptivedeficitscanbethereforeconnectedtolowbuffersaturationlevelsindicatingadiminishedprocessingofstimuliarisingfromthebody(i.e.,anorexianervosa,depression)butalsowithhighsaturationlevelsofnegativearousalstimuli(i.e.,chronicpain,anxiety)thatcanimpairtheperceptionofotherinputs.Furthermore,IBsmightalsodetectalteredprocessingbeforethepresenceofactualdeficits,providinganearlyindicatorofclinicalconditionsnotyetmanifested. TotestIBsmethodology,thestudyutilizedastreamofinteroceptiveparasympatheticstimulisentthroughtheC-TafferentsinthesecondarytouchsystemconnectedtothelaminaIspinothalamocorticalpathway,consequentiallymeasuringforsubjects’distortionsintimeperception.Stimuliweredeliveredusinganinteroceptivestimulatorspecificallydevelopedforthetask.Consideringaforementionedbodyofevidence,wehypothesizedthathealthysubjectswillunderestimatethedurationofparasympatheticC-Tinteroceptivestimuli.Furthermore,wehypothesizedthatIBsindex(i.e.,thedegreeofdurationestimationofinteroceptivestimulation)willpositivelycorrelatewithinteroceptiveaccuracy(IAc)asaproxyofinsula’sactivity(Pollatosetal.,2007a).Moreover,wehypothesizedthatseveralpsycho-physiologicalconditionsthatareknowntointerferewithAICactivitywillinfluenceIBsaswell,leadingtodistortionsintimeperceptionaccordinglytosubjects’sympatheticandparasympatheticbalance.Specifically,wehypothesizedthatdepressivesymptoms(Dunnetal.,2007,2010;Pollatosetal.,2009;PaulusandStein,2010;Wiebkingetal.,2015)andeatingdisordertendencies(Pollatosetal.,2008),willinterfereinanegativedirection,whileanxietysymptoms(WhymanandMoos,1967;Pollatosetal.,2009,2014;Dunnetal.,2010;PaulusandStein,2010;YooandLee,2015)andothersympatheticstimuli(Ogdenetal.,2015a)inapositiveone. Wetestedthesehypothesesonhealthysubjectsassessingforcommonvariablesconnectedtointeroceptivealterations.WeassessedforriskofanorexianervosathroughEDI-3(Garneretal.,1983)DriveforThinnesssubscale(Eshkevarietal.,2012),anddepressivesymptomsthroughBeckDepressionInventory(BDI-II;Becketal.,1961).Furthermore,weassessedanxietyfactorsthroughStateTraitAnxietyInventory(STAI;Spielbergeretal.,1970)andendogenousinteroceptivecardiacaccuracy(IAc)aswell(Schandry,1981). MaterialsandMethods Participants Aspartofalargerenlistingprocedureintheuniversitycampus,30femalesubjectswererecruitedthroughconsecutivesampling.Age(mean=25.87years,SD=6.616)andBMI(mean=20.827,SD=2.24)werecomparabletootherhealthysamplesusedininteroceptivestudies(Garfinkeletal.,2015).Samplewascomposedonlybyfemalesubjectstoavoidsomatosensorydifferencesinperceptionduetogenderrelatedfactors(Fillingimetal.,2009)assuggestedbyOgdenetal.(2015b).Furthermore,asolelyfemalesampleensurednodifferencesrelatedtoscalessensitivity,especiallyregardingeatingdisorderriskassessedbyEDI-3(Garneretal.,1983;Clausenetal.,2011;Eshkevarietal.,2012). Exclusioncriteriawerethepresenceofcurrentpsychologicalorphysicaldiagnoses,alterationsintactileperception(paraesthesia),allodyniaandheartrelatedconditions.Subjectswereaskedtoavoidpharmacologicalmedicationsinthe12hbeforetheexperimentandnicotineandcaffeineinthe2hbeforetheexperiment. ThisstudywascarriedoutinaccordancewiththerecommendationsoftheEthicsCommitteeofCatholicUniversityofSacredHeartofMilanwithwritteninformedconsentfromallsubjects.AllsubjectsgavewritteninformedconsentinaccordancewiththeDeclarationofHelsinki(2008).TheprotocolwasapprovedbytheEthicsCommitteeofCatholicUniversityofSacredHeartofMilan. Procedure Onarrival,subjectsreceivedinformationabouttheexperimentandproceededtogivewrittenconsent.Followingabriefanamnesticinterviewandaseriesofpsychologicalquestionnaires,subjectswereconnectedtoaportableECGdevicewithAg/AgClelectrodestoperformtheIActask.Attheendofthetask,electrodeswereremovedandsubjectsperformedIBs,audioandvideotasks.IBs,audioandvideotaskswerepresentedinrandomizedorder. PsychologicalAssessment Aftertheirarrivalsubjectstookparttoabriefanamnesticinterviewwithapsychologistspecializedinpsychopathologicalandpersonalityassessment.Afteranamnesticdatacollectiontheyperformedabatteryofquestionnaires.DepressivesymptomswereassessedthroughtheBDI-II(Becketal.,1961).BDI-IIisa21itemsself-reportquestionnairewithstrongliteraturesupport.AnxietysymptomswereassessedthroughthewellvalidatedSTAI(Spielbergeretal.,1970).STAIisa40-itemscalethatprovidedmeasureofstate(STAI-S)andtrait(STAI-T)anxiety.RisksforeatingdisorderswereassessedthroughEDI-3(Garneretal.,1983)risksubscales(Eshkevarietal.,2012).EDI-3risksubscalesassessedthreedifferentaspectofeatingdisordersrisk:DriveforThinness(DT),Bulimia(B)andBodyDissatisfaction(BD).Globalriskindex(EDRC)iscomposedsummingthescoresofthesethreesubscales. InteroceptiveAccuracy AnIAcscorewasestablishedwiththeSchandryheartbeattask(Schandry,1981)throughaportableECGunitsamplingat250Hz(Villarrubiaetal.,2014a,b;Stojanovićetal.,2015;Ševcíketal.,2015;HugengandKurniawan,2016)withAg/AgClelectrodes.Timeintervalswere25,35,45and100s.Accuracyindexwascalculatedwiththefollowingformula:1/4∑(1−(|recordedheartbeats−countedheartbeats|)/recordedheartbeats). WefocusedoncardiacinteroceptionnotonlybecauseSchandrytaskisconsideredthestandardmeasureforIAc,butalsobecauseCraig(2009)suggestedthattimeperceptionmightbefundamentallybaseduponcardiorespiratoryfunction. InteroceptiveStimulation TheInteroceptiveBuffersaturation(IBs)taskappliedinteroceptiveparasympatheticstimuliandaskedtheparticipantstoestimatethedurationofthesestimuli.Whilethereareseveraldifferentkindsofinteroceptivestimuli,forthegoalofthisstudyweusedlighttouchasprimaryparasympatheticinput(Ackerleyetal.,2014b).NeuroanatomicalevidenceidentifiedspecificCtactile(C-T)afferents,whichreportdirectlytotheAIC(Gordonetal.,2013)thatareexquisitelysensitivetolighttouch(Vallboetal.,1999;Ackerleyetal.,2014b).UnmyelinatedC-Tfibersselectivelyrespondtoslowtactilebrushingstimulibetween1cm/sand10cm/s(Crucianellietal.,2013;McGloneetal.,2014).Therefore,weusedaninstrumentexplicitlydevelopedtodistributepreciseC-Tstimuli(Figure1)andspecificallyprogrammedfortheIBstask. FIGURE1 Figure1.InteroceptiveStimulator. UnmyelinatedC-fibertactileafferentsareaspecifictypeoffibersthatcanbefoundinnotglabrousskin,constitutingasecondarytouchsystemthatprojectsintheAIC(Olaussonetal.,2002).Theyhavebeenspecificallyidentifiedinthefacialskin(Nordin,1990)andintheforearm(Vallboetal.,1995,1999;Wessbergetal.,2003).Theyarereceptivetolowforceandlowvelocitystrokesstimuli(Ackerleyetal.,2014a,b).TheyexhibitaparticularfiringbehaviorwithandinvertedUrelationshipwithstrokevelocity(Lökenetal.,2009),atendencytofatigue(Iggo,1960;Wessbergetal.,2003)andanafter-dischargepatternthatmaypresentsecondarycharacteristicsuchasdelayedacceleration(Vallboetal.,1995,1999). Forsuchreason,theinteroceptivestimulatorhasbeendesignedtoprovidecontinuousinteroceptivestimuliontheforearm,accountingforallC-Tafferentsfactors.C-Tafferentsshowedtheirmaximalmeanfiringfrequencyatslowstrokingvelocityof3cm/s(Ackerleyetal.,2014a),thustheinteroceptivestimulatorwassettomovethebrushtipattheexactspeedof3cm/s(±0.5cm/s).Thebrushtiphadanovalshapeareaof≈35mm2,tomatchthereceptivefieldofahumanC-Tafferent(Wessbergetal.,2003;LiljencrantzandOlausson,2014).Thebrushusedaconstantforce<2.5mNthatisreportedasC-Tthresholdbydifferentauthors(Nordin,1990;Vallboetal.,1999;Macefield,2009),measuredaccordingtoVallboetal.(1999).Furthermore,thebrushdesignhasbeenspecificallydevelopedtoavoidfatigueandinexcitability(Iggo,1960;Wessbergetal.,2003).Asamatteroffact,C-Tafferentsdecreasetheirfiringratetozeroafter5sofcontinuousstimulation(LiljencrantzandOlausson,2014).Therefore,thebrushtipmovedinacircularpattern(10cmoftotallength)atspecificvelocityof3cm/splusalinearvelocityof0.5cm/s.Consideringcircularpattern,receptivefieldarea,brushtipdimensions(8.6×4.1mm)andstrokesvelocity,asingleclusterofC-Tafferentsreceivedstimulationonlyfor≈0.3s(0.28s)withinasinglerevolution,widelybelowthe5slimit.Therestspanbetweenfieldstimuliallowedpossibleafter-dischargestobeexpressedwithintheactivetestduration.Furthermore,6srestingphasebetweensingletrialsand1minrestingphasebetweenstimulationblocksensuredanacceptablerecoveryoftheC-Tafferentfields.Delayedaccelerationdidnothaveaparticularrelevanceforourdesign,consideringthatisnotassociatedwithsubjectivesensations(Vallboetal.,1995,1999). InteroceptiveBufferSaturation(IBs)Index TheIBstaskisbasedontheTactileEstimationTask(TET)forspatialrecognition,buttheprotocolisadaptedtotemporalestimation(Krameretal.,2011),thusprovidingasimpleandnon-invasiveproceduretoassessthedegreeofestimationofdurationoftheinteroceptivestimulus.Thetaskconsistsindeliveringinteroceptivebrushstrokestosubject’sleftvolarforearm,andsuccessivelyaskingforverbaltimeestimation(VET)ofthetimepassed.ThechoiceofVET1isduetoitseffectivenessinprobingtimeperceptionrelatedtovisual,auditory(GilandDroit-Volet,2011)andtactiledimensions(Ogdenetal.,2015b)assessingforalterationsinpulserateswithinthepacemaker-accumulator(Mionietal.,2016).Previousstudiesfocuseduponinducingdistortionsintheinnerflowoftime,troughexternalinductivestimuli.Conversely,IBsindexwillusethenaturalflowoftimeinsidetheinteroceptivematrixtoprobethesaturationofinteroceptivebuffer. Previousstudiesregardinginteroceptivetouchutilizedstimulationatdifferentvelocity(>20cm/s)ordifferenttargetsites(i.e.,palmsofthehands)ascontrolprocedures.ThesecontroltasksrelyuponA-fibersstimulationandthesefibersareselectivelyactivatedbyhighvelocities/highpressurestimuliorselectivelypresentinthepalmofthehands(Ackerleyetal.,2014a,b;LiljencrantzandOlausson,2014;McGloneetal.,2014).Nonetheless,thesekindofcontrolconditionswerenotsuitableforthepresentstudydesignduetothefactthatperceiveddurationdependsonperceivedspeed(Tomassinietal.,2011),excludingthepossibilitytousefaststimuli(speed>20cm/s)ascontrolforinteroceptivetouch(Crucianellietal.,2013;Ackerleyetal.,2014a).Additionally,slowbrushstimulionthepalmofthehandsshowedhedonic(parasympathetic)velocity-independentvalence(Ackerleyetal.,2014b),whichcouldcompromiseIBstaskdesign.Moreover,differentclinicalsubjectsexhibiteddeficitsintactileperceptionconnectedtoA-fibers(Keizeretal.,2011,2012;Stantonetal.,2013;Catleyetal.,2014),thusA-fibersstimulationcannotbereliablyusedascontrolprocedureinperspectiveofIBsindex.Therefore,commonaudioandvisualtimeestimationtaskswereperformedascontrolproceduresuponseparatesensorymodalities. Subjectswereseatedinachairinacomfortableroomwhiletheclinicianexplainedtheexperiment.Theywereinstructedtogivea“subjectivetimeestimationofthestimulation”.Theywereinstructednottocountthetimepassing,topayattentiontothefeelingofthestimulationandtoclosetheireyesfortheentiredurationoftheprocedure.Toavoidcounting,cliniciansuggestedfocusingonthephysicalsensationofthestimulation.Subjectswereinstructedthatstimulicouldlastbetween“1and30sapproximately”,toavoidlevellingunderestimationeffect(Ogdenetal.,2015b).Subjectslaidtheirleftarmonthetableinfrontoftheclinician,bareskin(Tsakirisetal.,2011;Ackerleyetal.,2014b).Theclinicianbegantheexperimentstartingthetrainingblockonthestimulatorprogramdeliveringthreebrushstimuliof7,21,and15s,allowingthesubjecttofamiliarizewiththeprocedure.Nofeedbackswereprovidedforeffectivedurationsorforperformances.Aftereachstimulus,apauseof6sallowedcliniciantoaskthesubject,“Howmanysecondsdoyoufeelthestimuluslasted?”Afterthetrainingprocedure,clinicianstartedtheexperimentaltaskwithrandomizedtimedurations.IBsindextaskdeliveredstimulifrom8sto18s,atfixedintervals(8,10,12,14,16,18).Theexperimentaltaskprovidedsixrandomizedpredeterminedstimuliperblock,forthreerandomizedblocks.Partialaccuracyindexforeachtimeintervalwascalculatedwiththefollowingformula:1/3∑((timeestimation−realtime)/realtime).Totalindexwascalculatedasmeanofpartialindexes. AudioandVideoTimeEstimation Audioandvideoestimationtaskswerecommontasksfrequentlyusedandwelldescribedintheliterature.FollowingKrameretal.(2011),audiotaskpresentedaseriesofaudioamplitude-steadycomplextones.VideotaskreplicatedproceduresfromWeardenetal.(1998)substitutingtoneswitha4×4cmlightbluesquarepresentedonaniPad.Stimulidurationswere100,200,500,1000and3000msreplicatedsixtimeseachandpresentedinrandomorder.Audiotaskhadatrainingprocedureof1000,300and1500ms.Videotaskhadatrainingprocedureof1500,1000and500ms.Subjectswerenotinformedaboutdurationsorperformances(Weardenetal.,1998;Grondin,2010;Krameretal.,2011)buttheywereinformedthatstimulicouldlastbetween50msand4000ms(Ogdenetal.,2015b).Aftereachstimulussubjectswrotetheestimateddurationinmillisecondsonadatacollectiongrid.Accuracyindexeswerecalculatedwiththefollowingformula:1/6∑((timeestimation−realtime)/realtime).Totalindexwascalculatedasmeanofpartialindexes. StatisticalAnalyses ToverifyunderestimationofinteroceptiveC-Tstimuli,aseriesofonesamplet-testwereusedtodetermineifmeantimeestimationforeverysingleintervaldifferedsignificantlyfromrealtimevalues.Thesameprocedurehasalsobeenappliedtoaudioandvideomeanestimations. ArepeatedmeasuresANOVAwasrunforIBspartialindexesbetweenfirst,secondandlaststimulationblocktoverifythatIBstaskdidnothaveanyeffectonthebuffer. ArepeatedmeasuresANOVAwasrunforIBs,audioandvideoaccuracyindexestoidentifysignificantdifferencesbetweenaccuracyverbalestimationscoresindifferentsensorymodalities.Bonferroniposthocwasruntoidentifydifferencebetweengroups. Duetoknownlimitations(Dunnetal.,2010)inherentassessmentscalesused,differentfactorstructureshavebeenimplementedtobetterexplorerelationshipbetweenvariables.Specifically,analysesimplementedatwo-factorsstructureforBDI-IItoexploresomaticandcognitivedepressionfactors(Steeretal.,1999;Storchetal.,2004),andafour-factorsstructureforSTAItoexplorethepresenceandtheabsenceofanxietyelements,bothinstateandtraitdimensions(VigneauandCormier,2008).Furthermore,scatterplotgraphsandliteratureregardinginteractionbetweeninteroceptionanddepression(Dunnetal.,2007,2010;Pollatosetal.,2009)suggestedaquadraticnegativerelationshipbetweenBDI-IIsomaticfactorandIBs. Correlationanalyseswererunforvariablesofmaininterest.Moreover,amultipleregressionanalysiswasconductedwithIBsindexasdependentvariableandIAc,EDIDTsubscaleandBDI-IIsomaticsymptomsaspredictors. Followingliteraturesuggestions(Pollatosetal.,2009;Dunnetal.,2010),asecondmultipleregressionanalysiswasconductedwithIBsindexasdependentvariableandIAc,EDIDTsubscale,BDI-IIsomaticsymptomsandinteractionbetweenthesomaticcomponentofBDI-IIandthepositivefactorofSTAI-Saspredictors.Allvariableswerecenteredbeforeenteringtheregressionanalysis.ThepositivefactorofSTAI-SandthesomaticfactorofBDI-IIwerealsoz-standardizedbeforecalculatingtheinteractionterm.Allthelowleveltermswereleftintheregression,aspermethodologicalrecommendations(Aikenetal.,1991).Residualplotswerecheckedalongwithnormalityforobservedstandardizedandunstandardizedresiduals.ThesameregressionanalyseswerealsoconductedwithaudioandvideoaccuracyindexesinsubstitutionofIBsindex. Results SamplePsychologicalCharacteristics TotalsampleofN=30showedhighlevelsoftraitanxiety(mean=42.90,SD=9.732)andmoderatetohighlevelofstateanxiety(mean=35.80SD=7.406).Resultswerecomparabletootherpreviousstudies(Aktekinetal.,2001;Pollatosetal.,2007b,2009). Depressivesymptomswerehigher(mean=7.60,SD=5.157)thanprevioussamplesininteroceptivestudies(Pollatosetal.,2009)withsubjectsinrangeofmildclinicaldepression(Steeretal.,1999;Storchetal.,2004)allowingustomeaningfullyexplorerelationshipwiththisvariable. Nonetheless,depressivesymptomslevelswerecomparabletonormativedataforsimilarpopulations(Storchetal.,2004).EDI-3subscalesassessedmoderaterisksofEDspecificallyrelatedtoBD(mean=12.77,SD=7.938;BD)andDriveforThinness(mean=7.47,SD=6.564;DT).Severalsignificantcorrelationswerefoundbetweenpsychometricvariables.ResultsaresummarizedinTables1,2.ScatterplotdistributionsareprovidedinFigure2. TABLE1 Table1.Samplecharacteristicsandpsychologicalassessment. TABLE2 Table2.Correlationanalysesfornormallydistributedvariablesofmaininterest. FIGURE2 Figure2.Scatterplotdistributions.IBs,interoceptivebuffersaturationindex;IAc,interoceptivecardiacaccuracy;EDI_DT,EDIdriveforthinnessrisksubscale;Audio,Audioaccuracyindex;Video,videoaccuracyindex;BDI,BeckDepressioninventory;STAI_T,STAItraitanxiety;STAI_S,STAIstateanxiety. InteroceptiveAccuracy Interoceptivecardiacaccuracy(Schandry,1981)isameasureofheart-ratedetectionability,andthissensitivityhasbeencorrelatedwithactivationintheanteriorinsula(Pollatosetal.,2007a)andprovidedastandardmeasureofinteroceptiveawareness(Garfinkeletal.,2015).IAcmeanscorewas0.473(SD=0.231)andmedianwas0.50.IAcsignificantlypositivelycorrelatedwithIBsindex(r=0.504,p=0.005).ResultsaresummarizedinTable2. InteroceptiveBufferSaturationIndex Ashypothesized,healthysubjectssignificantlyunderestimateddurationsofinteroceptivestimuli.Meanscoreswerenormallydistributed.Aseriesofonesamplet-testshowedsignificantlyunderestimationofmeantimeperceptionofinteroceptivestimuliforalltimespans:8s(t(29)=−7.396,p<0.001,d=−1.35),10s(t(29)=−5.628,p<0.001,d=−1.02),12s(t(29)=−8.162,p<0.001,d=−1.49),14s(t(29)=−7.004,p<0.001,d=−1.27),16s(t(29)=−6.955,p<0.001,d=−1.27),18s(t(29)=−7.111,p<0.001,d=−1.298).ResultsaresummarizedinFigure3. FIGURE3 Figure3.Meantimeestimationsofinteroceptivetactilestimuliacrossdifferenttimespans.InteroceptiveBuffersaturation(IBs)VET,meanverbalinteroceptivetimeestimation.*p<0.05,**p<0.01,***p<0.001. Ashypothesized,IBstaskdidnothaveanymanipulativeeffectonthebuffer,confirmingtheeffectivenessofthetaskasassessmentinstrument.ArepeatedmeasuresANOVAshowednostatisticallysignificantdifferencesonpartialIBsindexesbetweenfirst,secondandlaststimulationblock(F(2,58)=0.142,p=0.868,ηp2=0.005).ResultsindicatedthatIBstaskdidnotaltersubjects’endogenousbaseline2. Ashypothesized,Pearson’scorrelationanalysesshowedasignificantlypositivelinearrelationshipbetweenIBsandIAc(r=0.504,p=0.005)andsignificantlynegativelinearrelationshipbetweenIBsandEDIDriveforThinness(r=−0.375,p=0.041).ResultsaresummarizedinTable2. Scatterplotandliterature(Dunnetal.,2007,2010)suggestedaquadraticrelationshipbetweendepressionandinteroceptiveprocessing.Ashypothesized,IBscorrelationwithaquadratictermofBDI-IIsomaticfactorapproachsignificanceforanegativerelationship(r=−0.358,p=0.052). AmultipleregressionanalysiswasconductedwithIBsindexasdependentvariableandIAc,EDIDriveforThinnessandquadratictransformationofBDI-IIsomaticfactoraspredictors.Sampledimensionassuredanadequatepower(Harrell,2015)fortheanalysis.Ashypothesized,variablessignificantlypredictedIBs(R2=0.443,F(3,26)=6.880,p<0.001).BetastandardizedcoefficientsindicatedthatIAcsignificantlypositivelypredictedIBs(β=0.420,p=0.009)whileEDIDriveforThinness(β=−0.306,p≤0.05)andsomaticdepressivefactorofBDI-II(β=−0.324,p≤0.05)3significantlynegativelypredictedsaturationlevelsintheinteroceptivebuffer. Toexplorerelationshipbetweenanxietyandinteroceptivebufferlevels,asecondmultipleregressionanalysiswasconductedwithIBsindexasdependentvariableandIAc,EDIDriveforThinness,BDI-IIsomaticquadratictransformationandinteractionbetweenthesomaticcomponentofBDI-IIandthepositiveSTAI-Sfactoraspredictors(Pollatosetal.,2009;Dunnetal.,2010;PaulusandStein,2010).VariablessignificantlypredictedIBs(R2=0.529,adjustedR2=0.406,F(6,23)=4.306,p<0.005).IAcpositivelypredictedsaturationlevelsinthebuffer(β=0.465,p=0.005)whileEDIDriveforThinness(β=−0.345,p=0.028)andBDI-IIsomaticfactor(β=−0.394,p=0.018)significantlynegativelypredictedIBs.InteractiontermbetweensomaticBDI-IIfactorandpositiveSTAI-Sfactorapproachedsignificance(β=0.318,p=0.083).Samplesizewasslightlyunderpoweredforthisregressionmodel,nonethelessmethodologicalrecommendationsindicatedthatmultipleregressionisquiterobusttosmallsampleifadjustedR2isconsideredinsubstitutionofR2(AustinandSteyerberg,2015). AudioandVideoTimeEstimation Onesamplet-testswereperformedbetweenaudioandvideotimeestimationsandrealtimespans.Wilcoxontestswereperformedfornot-normallydistributedvariables.Audioestimationsdidnotshowanystatisticallysignificantdifferencefor100,200,1000and3000mstimespans(p>0.05)comparedtorealtimespans.Subjectssignificantlyoverestimated500msaudiostimuli(t(29)=3.958,p<0.001,d=0.722).Videoestimationsdidnotshowanystatisticallysignificantdifferencefor100,500,1000mstimespans(p>0.05)comparedtorealtimespans.Subjectssignificantlyoverestimated200msvideostimuli(t(29)=2.242,p=0.033,d=0.409)andsignificantlyunderestimated3000msvideostimuli(t(29)=−3.032,p=0.005,d=−0.553).ResultsaresummarizedinFigure4.Onesamplet-testsforpairedsamplewereperformedbetweennormallydistributedaudioandvideotimeestimations.Wilcoxontestswereperformedfornot-normallydistributedvalues.Subjectsjudgedaudiostimulisignificantlylongerthanvideostimuliintimespans500,1000and3000ms(p=0.005).At-testforpairedsamplewasperformedbetweenaudioandvideoaccuracyindexeswithoutanystatisticallysignificantdifference(p=0.232).Audioandvideoaccuracyindexessignificantlypositivelycorrelated(r=0.588,p=0.001).ResultsaresummarizedinTable2.Regressionanalysesforaudio(p=0.490)andvideo(p=0.478)accuracyindexeswereperformedwithIAc,EDI_DTandBDI-IIsomaticfactor.Bothmodelsfailedtoreachstatisticalsignificance4. FIGURE4 Figure4.Meantimeestimationofaudioandvideostimuliacrossdifferenttimespans.AudioVET,meanverbalaudiotimeestimation;VideoVET,meanverbalvideotimeestimation.*p<0.05,**p<0.01,***p<0.001. RelationshipBetweenMeasures ArepeatedmeasuresANOVAwithaGreenhouse-GeissercorrectionwasperformedbetweenIBs,audioandvideoaccuracyindexes(factoraccuracy).Resultsreportedastatisticallysignificantdifferencebetweenaccuracyindexes(F(1.806,52.371)=16.037,p<0.001,ηp2=0.356).PosthoctestsusingBonferronicorrectionrevealedthatIBsaccuracyindexwassignificantlyunderestimatedcomparedtobothaudio(p<0.001)andvideo(p=0.003)accuracyindexes.Nostatisticallysignificantdifferenceswerefoundbetweenaudioandvideoaccuracyindexes(p=0.697)althoughaudiostimuli(mean=0.241,SD=0.080)werejudgedlongerthanvideoones(mean=0.126,SD=0.116). Severalstatisticallysignificantcorrelationswerefoundbetweenpsychometricvariables.ResultsaresummarizedinTable2. Discussion Thestudyexploredtheconnectionbetweentimeperceptionandinteroceptiveprocessing,proposingtheIBsindexasanovelconstructabletoprovideinformationaboutco-activeprocessingwithintheinteroceptivematrix(AIC;Craig,2002,2003,2009).TheIBsindexrelieduponaverbalestimationtaskofinteroceptivestimulisentthroughthesecondarytouchsystemconnectedtothelaminaIspinothalamocorticalpathway.Bidirectionaldistortionsinthesubjectivetimeperceptionofthesestimuliprovideinformationaboutsympatheticandparasympatheticactivityinthecortex.Theinteroceptivebufferisakeycomponentoftheinteroceptivematrixanditconstantlyprocessesandcomparesinformationarisingfromthebody,creatingbodilymeta-representationsshapedupontheasymmetricalrelationshipbetweentherightandtheleftinsula.Understandingsaturationlevelswithinthebuffercanprovidemeaningfulinsightaboutthisrelationship,whereashighlevelsofsaturationmayindicatelargeamountofsympatheticstimuliprocessedintherightinsula,whileoppositeresultsmayindicatethattheinteroceptivesystemisactuallyprocessingalowamountofhighsaliencesympatheticstimulifromthebody. Recentevidenceidentifieddeficitsintheinsulaconnectedwithradicallydifferentpsychopathologicalconditions.Chronicpain(Schaeferetal.,2012;Weissetal.,2014;Duscheketal.,2015),depression(Dunnetal.,2007,2010;Pollatosetal.,2009;Sprengelmeyeretal.,2011;SlizandHayley,2012;Stratmannetal.,2014),eatingdisorders(Pollatosetal.,2008;Fischeretal.,2016)andseveralotherconditions(Rossoetal.,2010;WylieandTregellas,2010;Hattonetal.,2012;Naqvietal.,2014)comprisedalterationswithintheinteroceptivenetwork.Nonetheless,itisnotclearlyunderstoodhowradicallydifferentconditionscanimpairtheinsulanetworkinasimilarway. Inourpreviouswork(DiLerniaetal.,2016a),wesuggestedthatIBsmayprovideabettermeasureofinteroceptiveprocessingcomparedtootheravailableindexes,becauseitpermitstodisentanglethedifferentialeffectsofinteroceptivealterationswithinthematrix.Conceptualizingthesealterationsintermsofbuffersaturationlevelssuggeststhathighlevelsofnegativearousingstimulimayimpairinteroceptiveprocessingthroughwidespreadinterferences(i.e.,interoceptivenoise),whereaslowstimuliprocessingmayreducebodyawareness(i.e.,interoceptivesilence),withbothconditionsleadingtodeficitsininteroceptiveperception. Thedesignofthestudydidnotincludenon-interoceptivecontrolstimuli,asexplainedinthe“MaterialsandMethods”section.Thiswasduetothenatureofthetaskweadopted,inwhichdifferentstimulationspeeds—whicharenecessarytoperformnoninteroceptivestimulation—wouldhaveyieldedincomparableresultsinatimeestimationtask.AsecondequallyimportantimplicationthatdefinedIBsdesignisalsothatinteroceptivetactilestimuliareneverexclusivelyrelatedtoC-Tfibers.NeurophysiologicalevidenceindicatedthateveryC-Trelatedstimulusalwayscomprisesaconcomitantactivationofmyelinatedtactilefibersprocessedbythesomatosensorycortex(Roudautetal.,2012).AlthoughamechanicalstimulationsuchastheonedeliveredbytheinteroceptivestimulatorcanbetunedtoproducemassivepeakactivationsinC-Ttactileafferents,everyinteroceptivetactilestimulusalwayspartiallyactivatesothermyelinatedfibers(Crucianellietal.,2013;Ackerleyetal.,2014a,b).Thereforeanon-interoceptivecontroltaskwouldhaveyieldedincomparable,albeitinformative,resultsfromIBsperspective.Thiswas,amongothers,oneoftheprominentreasonsaccordingtowhichwehavechosendifferentsensorymodalities(audioandvideo)ascontrolprocedures;alsoinagreementwithprotocolstostudyemotionsandtimeperceptionthatusuallycomparedresultsacrossdifferentunrelatedperceptivesystems.Nonetheless,consideringIBsdesign,onlycorrelationsbetweentheindexwiththeSchandry’staskandthequestionnairescanbeusedforaninterpretationregardinginteroceptiveprocesses. Ashypothesized,resultsconfirmedthathealthysubjectssignificantlyunderestimatedthedurationofparasympatheticinteroceptiveC-Tstimuli,comparedtoactualtimespans.ResultsconfirmedpreviousfindingsaboutC-Tprocessing(Leonardetal.,2014;Ogdenetal.,2015b),supportingthemethodologyusedinthepresentstudyandtheeffectivenessofthedevicedeveloped.Comparingourresultswithpreviousstudiesaboutsympatheticstimuli(Droit-Voletetal.,2011;Ogdenetal.,2015a)alsoendorsedCraig’semotionaltimeasymmetryframeworkandconsequentlytherationalebehindtheIBsindex.SignificantunderestimationofC-Tstimulisuggestedthathealthysubjectswithoutdominantsympatheticactivationtendtohaveareducedstimuliprocessingintherightinsulabuffer,matchingalowpulseratewithintheinteroceptiveinternalclockthereforeexperiencingacontractionintimeperceptionduetoalackofsalienceinbodilyarousal. Ashypothesized,IBsindexpositivelycorrelatedandwaspositivelypredictedbyinteroceptivecardiacaccuracy(IAc).ThisresultprovidedstrongsupportregardingthevalidityofIBsindexduetotheontologicalconnectionbetweenthebufferandthedegreeofactivationoftheinsula(Pollatosetal.,2007a).Nonetheless,correlationwassufficientlydistincttosuggestthatIBsindexandinteroceptivecardiacaccuracyaretwodifferent,albeitintertwined,constructs. IBsindexnegativelycorrelatedandwasnegativelypredictedbyEDI-3DriveforThinnesssubscale,suggestinganinterestinginsightregardingtheconnectionofthebufferwithbodyperception.IAcissignificantlyreducedinclinicalsubjectswithanorexianervosa(Pollatosetal.,2008);nonetheless,thenatureofthesedeficitsisnotreallyunderstoodeventhoughtheycanbeequallyfoundinradicallydifferentconditions(DiLerniaetal.,2016b).Inoursample,wefoundnosignificantcorrelationbetweenIAcandDriveforThinness,asexpectedforhealthyfemalesthatdidhaveneitherinteroceptivedeficitsnoranorexianervosa.Conversely,wefoundanegativecorrelationbetweenIBsandDriveforThinnessrisk,suggestingthatpredispositionforanorexianervosacanbeconnectedtolowsaturationlevelswithininteroceptivebuffer.Consideringourresults,lowsaturationlevelsinthebuffer(IBs)canbedescribedasahypo-saturationconditionwhereastheinteroceptivematrixprocessesloweramountsofhighsalience(i.e.,hunger,thirstetc.)stimulifromthebody.Thisimpairedprocessingactivitycanthereforeleadtopervasivealterationsinbodyperceptionand,ultimately,totheinteroceptivedeficitsandbodilydistortionsidentifiedinanorexianervosa(Pollatosetal.,2008).RecentfMRIevidenceshowedconsistentalterationsintheinsulanetworkbothonfunctional(Gaudioetal.,2015;Kerretal.,2016)andstructurallevel(Gaudioetal.,2017)indicatingimpairedprocessingofhighsaliencebodilystimuliinanorexianervosa(Wierengaetal.,2015),thereforesupportingourresults.Moreover,anorexicsubjectsshowednoimpairmentinatimedurationtaskconnectedtonon-interoceptiveneutraltactilestimuli(Spitonietal.,2015)fosteringtheconclusionthatIBsresultsarestrictlyconnectedtoprocessingwithininsulanetworkanditsrelationshipwithbodyperception. DepressionandanxietysymptomsdidnotshowadirectlinearcorrelationneitherwithIBsnorIAcaccuracyindexes,partiallydivergingfrompreviousliteratureresults(Pollatosetal.,2009).Thisdifferencecouldbedueeithertolimitedsampledimensioninourstudyortothelackofsensitivityofthetwoassessmentscalesweused,whichwouldbeinkeepingwithotherpreviousresults(Dunnetal.,2010).OurresultsindicateapooreffectivenessofBDI-IIandSTAIquestionnairestoassessinteroceptivedimensionsofdepressionandanxiety.Dunnetal.(2010)suggestedthatBDI-IIandSTAIquestionnairesmayhaveconfoundingoverlappingconstructs,moredirectlyconnectedtoglobalseveritymeasuresandthereforetheymightberelativelyinappropriatetomeasureinteroceptivecomponents.Ourfindings,relatedtoasmallalbeitrepresentativesample,confirmedthisperspectivealsothroughasignificantcorrelationbetweenthescales. Toeffectivelyassessbodilycomponentsofdepressionandanxiety,wethereforeimplementedseveralwellvalidatedfactorstructures.BDI-IIwasdividedintotwo-factorsconnectedtosomaticandcognitivecomponentsofdepression(Steeretal.,1999;Storchetal.,2004).STAIwasdividedintofour-factorsthatdescribedpositiveandnegativeitemsofanxiety,bothinstateandtraitconditions(VigneauandCormier,2008). Consequently,wefocusedonthesomaticfactorofBDI-IIandamoreaccurateanalysisidentifiedanot-linearnegativerelationship(Dunnetal.,2007)betweendepressivesomaticsymptomsandIBslevels.Ourresultssuggestedthatdepressionnegativelyinterferedwithsaturationlevelsinthebufferpromotingahypo-saturationconditioninwhichdepressivesomaticcomponentsappearedtoberelatedtoadiminishedprocessingofhighvalencearousingstimuliintherightinsula.Theseresultsarealsosupportedbypreviousneurophysiologicalevidencethatindicatedahypo-response(Wiebkingetal.,2015)alongwithreductioningraymattervolumeoftheinsula(Sprengelmeyeretal.,2011;Stratmannetal.,2014)inmajordepressivedisorders. Inourstudy,anxietymeasuredthroughSTAIquestionnairefailedtoprovidestatisticallysignificantresults.Thiscouldbeduetothesmallsamplesizebutmoreprobablytothegeneralconstructwithinthescalethatisprimarilyorientedtoglobalseveritymeasuresratherthananxietypositivearousalsymptoms(Dunnetal.,2010).Followingliteraturerecommendations(Pollatosetal.,2009;Dunnetal.,2010;PaulusandStein,2010),wenonethelessexploredtheinteractionbetweensomaticdepressivefactorandpositivestateanxietyfactor.Theinteractionapproachedapositivestatisticalsignificancesuggestingsomefurtherconsiderationsthat,nonetheless,mustbeconsideredwithcaution.Specifically,comorbiditybetweensomaticdepressiveandanxietysymptomsappearedtohaveamoderatelypositiveinfluenceuponsaturationlevelsinthebuffer,suggestingabiologicalprevalenceofsympatheticarousalpreferentiallyprocessedintherightinsula.Severalauthorsbackedthisperspectivethatreliedupontheroleofhigharousalstimuliintheoptimalfunctioningoftheorganism(Jänig,2008;Wiechetal.,2010;Uddin,2015)supportingthenotionofanemotionaladvantageinperceptionprocessingforhomeostaticregulatorypurposes,whichfosterenhancedattentiontobodilysignals(Ogdenetal.,2015a;YooandLee,2015). Interestingly,healthysubjectsdidnotsystematicallyunderestimatedurationsofaudioandvideostimuli.Moreover,asignificantdifferencebetweenIBs,audioandvideoaccuracyindexessuggestedsecondaryimplicationsregardingtimeperceptiontheories.Specifically,ourfindingsindicateddifferentpacemaker-accumulatorunits,connectedtodifferentsensorymodalities.Audioandvideoindexesshowedapositivesignificantcorrelationbetweeneachother,butfailedtosignificantlycorrelatetointeroceptivetimeperceptionindicatingseparatedtimeprocessingpathways.Moreover,regressionanalysisalsofailedtobesignificantfortheaudioandvideoaccuracyindexes,suggestingthataudioandvideotimeestimationprobablyreferredtodifferentprocessingcircuits,partiallydistinctfromtheinteroceptivesystem.Theseresultsaresupportedbyrecentliterature(Mionietal.,2016)accordingtowhichtimeperceptionindepressionshowedinconclusiveresults(ThonesandOberfeld,2015)alongwithnot-inductivetimestudiesinanxietythatalsoprovidedmixedresults(Lueck,2007;Brown,2016). Apossibleexplanationforthesignificantdifferencebetweenaudio,videoandIBsaccuracyindexescanberelatedtothedifferenttimespansused,suggestingthattimeestimationintherangeofmillisecondsisnotsusceptibletothesamepsycho-physiologicalcomponentsthatcontributetoIBsindex.Nonetheless,differentstudiesusedtimespanswithinmillisecondsrangetoidentifyaparasympathetic(Ogdenetal.,2015b)andsympathetic(Droit-VoletandMeck,2007;GilandDroit-Volet,2011,2012;Droit-Voletetal.,2013;Ogdenetal.,2015a;ThonesandOberfeld,2015;YooandLee,2015;Mionietal.,2016)influenceupontheinternalclock.Moreover,timespansintherangefromsecondstosubsecondshaveoftenbeenconsideredconnectedtothesametimeperceptionmechanisms(Church,1984)whereasevidencefromliteraturefoundnodifferenceintimeintervalsasafactor(Macaretal.,2002).Furthermore,evidencefromcomputationalneurosciencesuggestedthattimeestimationsfrommillisecondtosecondsrelyuponthesameencodingprocessingpatterns(KarmarkarandBuonomano,2007).KarmarkarandBuonomano(2007)alsosuggestedthatshorttimeintervals—astheloweronesintheaudioandvideoestimationtasks—arealsoprobablyconnectedtoanonlineartimemetricencodedinlocalneuralnetworksandthereforetheyshouldbeevenmoresensibletothepsychophysiologicalvariablesthatcontributetoIBsindex.Inasimilarmanner,Craigsuggestedthattheinteroceptivenetworkmight“provideabasisforthehumancapacitytoperceiveandestimatetimeintervalsintherangeofsecondstosubseconds”(Craig,2009)alsosupportingthepossibilitytocoherentlyexploredifferenttimespans.Thisevidencesuggestedthattimespansinmillisecondsrangeshouldbesensibletoalterationsinbodilyprocessing,thusexcludingthedurationofthestimuliasafactorandallowingthedesignofthestudytoselectthemostappropriateandthemostsensibletimeintervalsforeachsensorymodality. Consideringourresults,itisthereforepossiblethattimeperceptionisprocesseddifferentlydependingonthesensorymodalityinvestigatedandthattimeinformationcollectedbyseveraldifferentinternalclocksaresub-sequentiallymergedinaglobaltimeperceptionawareness,composedbydifferentelementsaccordingtotheircontextualsalience.Thishypothesisremainstobetestedinfuturestudies. InteroceptiveBuffer,ActiveInferenceandPredictiveCoding Fromamoreformal,predictivecoding(orinteroceptiveinference)perspective,buffersaturationcanbeunderstoodintermsofwell-knownmechanismsofprecision(orgain)controlduring(Bayesian)inference.Oneessentialroleofinteroceptiveinferenceisintegratingvariousinteroceptivesignalstoformanestimateofthestateofthebody(e.g.,heartrateaswellasgastricandrespiratorysignalstoassessmomentaryfatigueorfear),basedonwhichtheorganismcantakeadaptiveaction(Pezzulo,2014;Pezzuloetal.,2015).Importantly,whileformingthisestimate,thereliabilityofalltheinteroceptivesignals(aswellasofpriorinformation)mustbeevaluated,too;signalsthathavehighersalienceorprecision(orloweruncertainty)areweightedmoreandhavehigherimpactontheinference,whereaslower-precisionsignalshavelesserimpactandinsomecasescanbealsodisattendedorignored—thusimplementingaformofprecision(orgain)controloverinteroceptiveprocessing.Withinthisframework,signalsthatarehighlysalientorhavehighbehavioralsignificance(e.g.,threats)wouldbeplausiblyaffordedahighgain,hencedominatingtheinferenceandreducingtheimpactofothersignals(aka,saturatingthebuffer).Thisisingeneralanadaptivemechanism,whichwouldaffordprioritytothemostimportantsignals;butinsomecases,itcanbecomemaladaptive.Forexample,expectingtheenvironmenttobetoovolatilemightleadtoover-samplingit(like,e.g.,inanxiety)andtoasystematicoverestimationoftheprecisionofsensoryandinteroceptivesignals.Conversely,otherpathologicalconditions(e.g.,anorexia)maybelinkedtointeroceptiveprecisiondown-regulation,andthustoasystematicallydiminishedprocessingofstimuliarisingfromthebody(aka,silencingoneofthechannelsofthebufferratherthansaturatingit).Thisframeworkwouldshedlightonthedifferentwaysinteroceptiveinferencecanbecomeaberrant,due(forexample)tothefallaciousup-ordown-regulationoftheprecisionofinteroceptive(orother)signals—andhowthisinturnwouldproducemaladaptivebehaviorthatisspecificofdifferentclinicalconditions(Fristonetal.,2014). ClinicalImplicationofInteroceptiveBufferSaturation IBsindexcanprovideinsightregardinginteroceptivealterationsinapparentlydifferentconditions,nonethelessthisknowledgemayserveotherpurposesbesidessimpleassessment.Specifically,pathologicalconditionswithanhyper-saturatedstatusmightpresentalterationsinbodilysignalsconnectedtoanoverflowofsympathetic-relatedstimuliinthebuffer(i.e.,interoceptivenoise)loweringtheaccesstootherinteroceptiveinformation(DiLerniaetal.,2016a).Conversely,hypo-saturatedconditionsmaybeconnectedtoageneralimpairmentofwholestimuliprocessingthatlimitedinputs(i.e.,interoceptivesilence)fromactuallyreachingAIC. Ifconfirmed,thisperspectivemayhaveimportantimplicationsbeyondassessment,foreseeingtheconceptof“interoceptivetreatments”.Asamatteroffact,clinicalconditions(i.e.,anxietyorchronicpain)characterizedbyanoverflowofsympatheticrelatedinteroceptivestimuli(i.e.,hyper-saturation)maybenefitfromaninteroceptiveparasympatheticstimulationwithlowvariancetopromoteco-activationoftheleftinsula,reducingprocessingintherightAICwiththeultimategoalofcontrastingsymptomsseverity.Conversely,ifaclinicalconditionwillshowhypo-saturationduetoablockthatimpairsinteroceptiveinformationtoactuallyreachtheinsula(i.e.,depressionoranorexianervosa),interoceptivetreatmentsbaseduponinteroceptivestimuliwithhighvariancecouldprovideawaytofosterhighsalienceprocessingintherightAIC,restoringacorrectaccesstobodilyinformation.Ifconfirmed,albeithighlytheoretical,theseperspectivescanprovideinnovativeinsightsintreatmentfields,forseveraldifferentpsychopathologicalconditions. Limitations Severallimitationsimpairedstudydesignandresults.Samplesizewasreduced,nonethelessconsecutivesamplingprovidedhighlyinformativedata,andliteratureconfirmedanadequatestatisticalpower(AustinandSteyerberg,2015;Harrell,2015)relatedtonumberofsubjectsandstatisticalanalysesperformed. Asolelyfemalesampleensuredcomparableresultsonsomatosensoryrelatedtests(Fillingimetal.,2009)andpsychologicalassessment(Garneretal.,1983;Clausenetal.,2011;Eshkevarietal.,2012)neverthelessthedesignofthestudydidnotexploreIBsinmalepopulation. Thestudyutilizedaudioandvideostimuliascontrolproceduresduetothefactthatsomaticnon-interoceptiveconditions(i.e.,differentvelocitiesordifferentbodylocations)werenotsuitablewithinIBsdesign.Thischoiceallowedustoexploretimeperceptionupondifferentsensorymodalities.However,theabsenceofsomaticcontrolstimulididnotallowustoexplorepossibledifferencesinsomaticnon-interoceptiveprocessing.Moreover,interoceptivestimulationalwayspresentsconcomitantactivationofAβreceptors(Ackerleyetal.,2014a,b;Ogdenetal.,2015b)thereforenon-interoceptivestimuliwithaparasympatheticcomponentmaycorrelatewithIBsindex.Theseproblemsmightbeaddressedbyusingreliablesomaticcontrols,suchasvibrationalstimulithatspecificallytarget(LiljencrantzandOlausson,2014)Paciniancorpusclesatafrequencybetween150Hzand–300Hz(Roudautetal.,2012).Nonetheless,thiskindofstimulationrequiresaspecificallydesignedprototypenotavailableduringthisstudy. BDI-IIandSTAIquestionnairespresentedseverallimitationsregardingconstructmeasures,overlappingtheoreticalrelevance,andeffectivenesstomeasureinteroceptivecomponents(Dunnetal.,2010).Inourstudy,theselimitationswerepartiallyaddressedthroughseveralfactorstructuresnonethelessadifferentmodelofanxietyanddepressioncouldprovidebetterassessmentofinteroceptivedimensions.Specifically,thetripartitemodelofmooddisorders(ClarkandWatson,1991)mightprovideamoreeffectiveframeworkforinteroceptiverelatedcomponents.ThismodelreliesupontheMoodandAnxietySymptomQuestionnaire(MASQ-S;Watsonetal.,1995;KeoghandReidy,2000)toassessseveraldimensionsconnectedtosymptomsofnonspecificgeneraldistress,symptomsspecifictodepressionandsymptomsspecifictoanxiety.Specifically,MASQ-SsubscalesAnxiousArousal(17items)andAnhedonicDepression(22items)mightprovidebetterresultscomparedtoglobalseveritymeasuresreportedbyBDI-IIandSTAI.Unfortunately,Italianvalidationoftheinstrumentwasnotavailableatthetimeofthisstudy. Lastly,literaturesuggestedthatbeliefandexpectationscanlimitSchandrytaskvaliditytoassessforinteroceptivecardiacawareness(Ringetal.,2015).Nonetheless,heartbeatperceptiontask(Schandry,1981)remainedthemostvalidateandmostreliableinstrumentforIAcassessment(Pollatosetal.,2007a;CritchleyandGarfinkel,2015;Garfinkeletal.,2015). ConclusionandFutureDirections Ourresultsprovidedapowerfultheoreticalinsightregardingtherelationshipbetweentimeperception,interoceptiveprocessingandpsycho-physiologicalconditions.DistortionsininteroceptivetimeperceptionrecordedbyIBsindexappearedtobefunctionofdifferentsympatheticandparasympatheticco-activationprocesseswithininsulacortex. Nonetheless,differentquestionsremaintobeexplored,promotingfutureresearchdirections.Specifically,itwillbefundamentaltotestIBsindexproposalondifferentpathologicalconditionsconnectedtoinsuladeficits,suchaschronicpain,anorexianervosaanddepressionalsocomparingresultstohealthysubjects’performances. Furthermore,IBsresultsmaysupporttheconceptof“interoceptivetreatments”asclinicalapplicationsforanewnon-pharmacologicaloptiontotreatavarietyofdisorderscharacterizedbyinteroceptivenetworkalterations.Thesekindsoftreatments,tailoreduponspecificdysregulationsoftheinsulacortex,mightprovidepowerfulinstrumentstoreducesymptomsseverityinthoseconditionsthatareresistanttopharmacologicalmedications,withoutanysideeffectorinteractionwithconcomitanttherapies(DiLerniaetal.,2016a;Rivaetal.,2016,2017).Interoceptivetreatmentsmaythereforeprovethemselvesaneffectiveoptiontopromoteabalancedfunctioningoftheorganismbothinclinicalandhealthypopulation,openingabrandnewfieldofmedicineandneuroscience. AuthorContributions DDL:conceptualization,investigation,writing—originaldraft,hardwareandsoftwaredevelopment.DDLandSS:methodology.SS,GP,EP,PCandGR:writing—review,editingandsupervision. ConflictofInterestStatement Theauthorsdeclarethattheresearchwasconductedintheabsenceofanycommercialorfinancialrelationshipsthatcouldbeconstruedasapotentialconflictofinterest. Acknowledgments SSandGRwerefundedbytheMIURPRINresearchproject“Unlockingthememoryofthebody:VirtualRealityinAnorexiaNervosa”(201597WTTM). Footnotes ^Ontheotherhand,wedismissedtimeproductionandreproductiontasksbecausethatareusuallydependentuponattentionandworkingmemoryprocesses“ratherthanvariationininternalclock”(Mionietal.,2016). ^IBsisproposedasanassessinginstrument;thereforewecheckedifthestimulationhadanykindofeffectuponsubject’stemporalperception.Weverifiedthisbyconfrontingmeanestimationsofthefirststimulationblockwithmeanestimationsonthelaststimulationblock.Aneffectoftheinteroceptivestimulationwouldhaveproducedvariationsintemporalestimationacrossblocks.Sincewedidnotobservesignificativedifferencesintimeestimationbetweenfirstandlaststimuli,acrossalltheprotocol,weconcludedthatthestimulationitselfdidnothaveanyimpactonsubject’sendogenoustimeperception. ^BDI-IIsomaticfactorquadratictransformation(β=−0.324,p=0.037)|ModelwithuntransformedtermR2=0.461,F(4,25)=5.337,p<0.003,IAcβ=0.420,p=0.01,EDI_DTβ=−0.308,p=0.05,BDI-IIsomaticuntransformedtermβ=−0.136,p=0.369,BDI-IIsomaticquadratictransformedtermβ=−0.303,p=0.053.ResidualsK-S>0.200,S-W=0.928. ^Modelwithuntransformedterm:Audioaccuracyindexp=0.611,Videoaccuracyindexp=0.288. References Ackerley,R.,BacklundWasling,H.,Liljencrantz,J.,Olausson,H.,Johnson,R.D.,andWessberg,J.(2014a).HumanC-tactileafferentsaretunedtothetemperatureofaskin-strokingcaress.J.Neurosci.34,2879–2883.doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2847-13.2014 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Ackerley,R.,Carlsson,I.,Wester,H.,Olausson,H.,andBacklundWasling,H.(2014b).Touchperceptionsacrossskinsites:differencesbetweensensitivity,directiondiscriminationandpleasantness.Front.Behav.Neurosci.8:54.doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00054 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Aiken,L.S.,West,S.G.,andReno,R.R.(1991).MultipleRegression:TestingandInterpretingInteractions.NewYork,NY:Sage. GoogleScholar Aktekin,M.,Karaman,T.,Senol,Y.Y.,Erdem,S.,Erengin,H.,andAkaydin,M.(2001).Anxiety,depressionandstressfullifeeventsamongmedicalstudents:aprospectivestudyinAntalya,Turkey.Med.Educ.35,12–17.doi:10.1111/j.1365-2923.2001.00726.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Angrilli,A.,Cherubini,P.,Pavese,A.,andManfredini,S.(1997).Theinfluenceofaffectivefactorsontimeperception.Percept.Psychophys.59,972–982.doi:10.3758/bf03205512 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Austin,P.C.,andSteyerberg,E.W.(2015).Thenumberofsubjectspervariablerequiredinlinearregressionanalyses.J.Clin.Epidemiol.68,627–636.doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.12.014 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Beck,A.T.,Ward,C.H.,Mendelson,M.,Mock,J.,andErbaugh,J.(1961).Aninventoryformeasuringdepression.Arch.Gen.Psychiatry4,561–571.doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Brown,J.(2016).ARelationshipBetweenTimePerceptionandState-Anxiety.(AmericanJournalofUndergraduateResearch,AJUR)(Onlineversion). GoogleScholar Buetti,S.,andLleras,A.(2012).Perceivingcontroloveraversiveandfearfuleventscanalterhowweexperiencethoseevents:aninvestigationoftimeperceptioninspider-fearfulindividuals.Front.Psychol.3:337.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00337 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Buonomano,D.V.(2014).Neuraldynamicsbasedtiminginthesubsecondtosecondsrange.Adv.Exp.Med.Biol.829,101–117.doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-1782-2_6 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Buonomano,D.V.,andLaje,R.(2010).Populationclocks:motortimingwithneuraldynamics.TrendsCogn.Sci.14,520–527.doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.002 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Burle,B.,andCasini,L.(2001).Dissociationbetweenactivationandattentioneffectsintimeestimation:implicationsforinternalclockmodels.J.Exp.Psychol.Hum.Percept.Perform.27,195–205.doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.1.195 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Campbell,L.A.,andBryant,R.A.(2007).Howtimeflies:astudyofnoviceskydivers.Behav.Res.Ther.45,1389–1392.doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.011 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Catley,M.J.,O’Connell,N.E.,Berryman,C.,Ayhan,F.F.,andMoseley,G.L.(2014).Istactileacuityalteredinpeoplewithchronicpain?asystematicreviewandmeta-analysis.J.Pain15,985–1000.doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2014.06.009 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Church,R.M.(1984).Propertiesoftheinternalclock.Ann.NYAcad.Sci.423,566–582.doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23459.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Clark,L.A.,andWatson,D.(1991).Tripartitemodelofanxietyanddepression:psychometricevidenceandtaxonomicimplications.J.Abnorm.Psychol.100,316–336.doi:10.1037//0021-843x.100.3.316 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Clausen,L.,Rosenvinge,J.H.,Friborg,O.,andRokkedal,K.(2011).Validatingtheeatingdisorderinventory-3(EDI-3):acomparisonbetween561femaleeatingdisorderspatientsand878femalesfromthegeneralpopulation.J.Psychopathol.Behav.Assess.33,101–110.doi:10.1007/s10862-010-9207-4 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Coull,J.T.(2004).fMRIstudiesoftemporalattention:allocatingattentionwithin,ortowards,time.Cogn.BrainRes.21,216–226.doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.02.011 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Craig,A.D.(2002).Howdoyoufeel?Interoception:thesenseofthephysiologicalconditionofthebody.Nat.Rev.Neurosci.3,655–666.doi:10.1038/nrn894 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Craig,A.D.(2003).Interoception:thesenseofthephysiologicalconditionofthebody.Curr.Opin.Neurobiol.13,500–505.doi:10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Craig,A.D.(2009).Emotionalmomentsacrosstime:apossibleneuralbasisfortimeperceptionintheanteriorinsula.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.BBiol.Sci.364,1933–1942.doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0008 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Critchley,H.D.,andGarfinkel,S.N.(2015).Interactionsbetweenvisceralafferentsignalingandstimulusprocessing.Front.Neurosci.9:286.doi:10.3389/fnins.2015.00286 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Crucianelli,L.,Metcalf,N.K.,Fotopoulou,A.K.,andJenkinson,P.M.(2013).Bodilypleasurematters:velocityoftouchmodulatesbodyownershipduringtherubberhandillusion.Front.Psychol.4:703.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00703 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar DiLernia,D.,Serino,S.,Cipresso,P.,andRiva,G.(2016a).GhostsintheMachine.Interoceptivemodelingforchronicpaintreatment.Front.Neurosci.10:314.doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00314 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar DiLernia,D.,Serino,S.,andRiva,G.(2016b).Paininthebody.Alteredinteroceptioninchronicpainconditions:asystematicreview.Neurosci.Biobehav.Rev.71,328–341.doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.015 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Droit-Volet,S.,Fayolle,S.L.,andGil,S.(2011).Emotionandtimeperception:effectsoffilm-inducedmood.Front.Integr.Neurosci.5:33.doi:10.3389/fnint.2011.00033 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Droit-Volet,S.,andGil,S.(2009).Thetime-emotionparadox.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.BBiol.Sci.364,1943–1953.doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0013 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Droit-Volet,S.,andMeck,W.H.(2007).Howemotionscolourourperceptionoftime.TrendsCogn.Sci.11,504–513.doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Droit-Volet,S.,Ramos,D.,Bueno,J.L.,andBigand,E.(2013).Music,emotion,andtimeperception:theinfluenceofsubjectiveemotionalvalenceandarousal?Front.Psychol.4:417.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00417 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Dunn,B.D.,Dalgleish,T.,Ogilvie,A.D.,andLawrence,A.D.(2007).Heartbeatperceptionindepression.Behav.Res.Ther.45,1921–1930.doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.008 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Dunn,B.D.,Stefanovitch,I.,Evans,D.,Oliver,C.,Hawkins,A.,andDalgleish,T.(2010).Canyoufeelthebeat?Interoceptiveawarenessisaninteractivefunctionofanxiety-anddepression-specificsymptomdimensions.Behav.Res.Ther.48,1133–1138.doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.006 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Duschek,S.,Montoro,C.I.,andReyesDelPaso,G.A.(2015).Diminishedinteroceptiveawarenessinfibromyalgiasyndrome.Behav.Med.43,100–107.doi:10.1080/08964289.2015.1094442 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Eshkevari,E.,Rieger,E.,Longo,M.R.,Haggard,P.,andTreasure,J.(2012).Increasedplasticityofthebodilyselfineatingdisorders.Psychol.Med.42,819–828.doi:10.1017/s0033291711002091 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Fillingim,R.B.,King,C.D.,Ribeiro-Dasilva,M.C.,Rahim-Williams,B.,andRiley,J.L.(2009).Sex,gender,andpain:areviewofrecentclinicalandexperimentalfindings.J.Pain10,447–485.doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Fischer,D.,Berberich,G.,Zaudig,M.,Krauseneck,T.,Weiss,S.,andPollatos,O.(2016).Interoceptiveprocessesinanorexianervosainthetimecourseofcognitive-behavioraltherapy:apilotstudy.Front.Psychiatry7:199.doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00199 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Friston,K.(2009).Thefree-energyprinciple:aroughguidetothebrain?TrendsCogn.Sci.13,293–301.doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.005 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Friston,K.(2010).Thefree-energyprinciple:aunifiedbraintheory?Nat.Rev.Neurosci.11,127–138.doi:10.1038/nrn2787 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Friston,K.,Mattout,J.,andKilner,J.(2011).Actionunderstandingandactiveinference.Biol.Cybern.104,137–160.doi:10.1007/s00422-011-0424-z PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Friston,K.J.,Stephan,K.E.,Montague,R.,andDolan,R.J.(2014).Computationalpsychiatry:thebrainasaphantasticorgan.LancetPsychiatry1,148–158.doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(14)70275-5 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Garfinkel,S.N.,Seth,A.K.,Barrett,A.B.,Suzuki,K.,andCritchley,H.D.(2015).Knowingyourownheart:distinguishinginteroceptiveaccuracyfrominteroceptiveawareness.Biol.Psychol.104,65–74.doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Garner,D.M.,Olmstead,M.P.,andPolivy,J.(1983).Developmentandvalidationofamultidimensionaleatingdisorderinventoryforanorexianervosaandbulimia.Int.J.Eat.Disord.2,15–34.doi:10.1002/1098-108x(198321)2:2<15::aid-eat2260020203>3.0.co;2-6 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gaudio,S.,Piervincenzi,C.,BeomonteZobel,B.,RomanaMontecchi,F.,Riva,G.,Carducci,F.,etal.(2015).Alteredrestingstatefunctionalconnectivityofanteriorcingulatecortexindrugnaïveadolescentsattheearlieststagesofanorexianervosa.Sci.Rep.5:10818.doi:10.1038/srep10818 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gaudio,S.,Quattrocchi,C.C.,Piervincenzi,C.,Zobel,B.B.,Montecchi,F.R.,Dakanalis,A.,etal.(2017).Whitematterabnormalitiesintreatment-naiveadolescentsattheearlieststagesofAnorexiaNervosa:adiffusiontensorimagingstudy.PsychiatryRes.266,138–145.doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.06.011 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gavazzi,G.,Bisio,A.,andPozzo,T.(2013).Timeperceptionofvisualmotionistunedbythemotorrepresentationofhumanactions.Sci.Rep.3:1168.doi:10.1038/srep01168 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gibbon,J.,Church,R.M.,andMeck,W.H.(1984).Scalartiminginmemory.Ann.NYAcad.Sci.423,52–77.doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gil,S.,andDroit-Volet,S.(2011).“Timefliesinthepresenceofangryfaces”anddependingonthetemporaltaskused!.ActaPsychol136,354–362.doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.12.010 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gil,S.,andDroit-Volet,S.(2012).Emotionaltimedistortions:thefundamentalroleofarousal.Cogn.Emot.26,847–862.doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.625401 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gordon,I.,Voos,A.C.,Bennett,R.H.,Bolling,D.Z.,Pelphrey,K.A.,andKaiser,M.D.(2013).Brainmechanismsforprocessingaffectivetouch.Hum.BrainMapp.34,914–922.doi:10.1002/hbm.21480 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Grecucci,A.,Giorgetta,C.,Rattin,A.,Guerreschi,C.,Sanfey,A.G.,andBonini,N.(2014).Timedevoursthings:howimpulsivityandtimeaffecttemporaldecisionsinpathologicalgamblers.PLoSOne9:e109197.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0109197 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Grondin,S.(2010).Timingandtimeperception:areviewofrecentbehavioralandneurosciencefindingsandtheoreticaldirections.Atten.Percept.Psychophys.72,561–582.doi:10.3758/app.72.3.561 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gu,X.,andFitzGerald,T.H.(2014).Interoceptiveinference:homeostasisanddecision-making.TrendsCogn.Sci.18,269–270.doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.001 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Gu,X.,Hof,P.R.,Friston,K.J.,andFan,J.(2013).Anteriorinsularcortexandemotionalawareness.J.Comp.Neurol.521,3371–3388.doi:10.1002/cne.23368 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Harrell,F.(2015).RegressionModelingStrategies:WithApplicationstoLinearModels,LogisticandOrdinalRegressionandSurvivalAnalysis.Heidelberg:Springer. GoogleScholar Hatton,S.N.,Lagopoulos,J.,Hermens,D.F.,Naismith,S.L.,Bennett,M.R.,andHickie,I.B.(2012).Correlatinganteriorinsulagraymattervolumechangesinyoungpeoplewithclinicalandneurocognitiveoutcomes:anMRIstudy.BMCPsychiatry12:45.doi:10.1186/1471-244x-12-45 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Hugeng,H.,andKurniawan,R.(2016).Developmentofthe‘Healthcor’systemasacardiacdisorderssymptomsdetectorusinganexpertsystembasedonarduinouno.Int.J.Technol.7,78–87.doi:10.14716/ijtech.v7i1.1575 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Iggo,A.(1960).CutaneousmechanoreceptorswithafferentCfibres.J.Physiol.152,337–353.doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1960.sp006491 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Jänig,W.(2008).IntegrativeActionoftheAutonomicNervousSystem:NeurobiologyofHomeostasis.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress. GoogleScholar Karmarkar,U.R.,andBuonomano,D.V.(2007).Timingintheabsenceofclocks:encodingtimeinneuralnetworkstates.Neuron53,427–438.doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.006 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Keizer,A.,Smeets,M.A.,Dijkerman,H.C.,vandenHout,M.,Klugkist,I.,vanElburg,A.,etal.(2011).Tactilebodyimagedisturbanceinanorexianervosa.PsychiatryRes.190,115–120.doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.031 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Keizer,A.,Smeets,M.A.,Dijkerman,H.C.,vanElburg,A.,andPostma,A.(2012).AberrantsomatosensoryperceptioninAnorexiaNervosa.PsychiatryRes.200,530–537.doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.05.001 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Keogh,E.,andReidy,J.(2000).ExploringthefactorstructureoftheMoodandAnxietySymptomQuestionnaire(MASQ).J.Pers.Assess.74,106–125.doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA740108 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kerr,K.L.,Moseman,S.E.,Avery,J.A.,Bodurka,J.,Zucker,N.L.,andSimmons,W.K.(2016).Alteredinsulaactivityduringvisceralinteroceptioninweight-restoredpatientswithanorexianervosa.Neuropsychopharmacology41,521–528.doi:10.1038/npp.2015.174 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kramer,P.,Bressan,P.,andGrassi,M.(2011).Timeestimationpredictsmathematicalintelligence.PLoSOne6:e28621.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0028621 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Kranjec,A.,andChatterjee,A.(2010).Aretemporalconceptsembodied?Achallengeforcognitiveneuroscience.Front.Psychol.1:240.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00240 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Leonard,J.A.,Berkowitz,T.,andShusterman,A.(2014).Theeffectoffriendlytouchondelay-of-gratificationinpreschoolchildren.Q.J.Exp.Psychol.67,2123–2133.doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.907325 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Lewis,P.A.,andMiall,R.(2006).Arighthemisphericprefrontalsystemforcognitivetimemeasurement.Behav.Processes71,226–234.doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2005.12.009 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Liljencrantz,J.,andOlausson,H.(2014).TactileCfibersandtheircontributionstopleasantsensationsandtotactileallodynia.Front.Behav.Neurosci.8:37.doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00037 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Livesey,A.C.,Wall,M.B.,andSmith,A.T.(2007).Timeperception:manipulationoftaskdifficultydissociatesclockfunctionsfromothercognitivedemands.Neuropsychologia45,321–331.doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.033 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Löken,L.S.,Wessberg,J.,Morrison,I.,McGlone,F.,andOlausson,H.(2009).Codingofpleasanttouchbyunmyelinatedafferentsinhumans.Nat.Neurosci.12,547–548.doi:10.1038/nn.2312 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Lueck,M.D.(2007).Anxietylevels:dotheyinfluencetheperceptionoftime?J.Undergrad.Res.X,1–5. GoogleScholar Lui,M.A.,Penney,T.B.,andSchirmer,A.(2011).Emotioneffectsontiming:attentionversuspacemakeraccounts.PLoSOne6:e21829.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0021829 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Macar,F.,Lejeune,H.,Bonnet,M.,Ferrara,A.,Pouthas,V.,Vidal,F.,etal.(2002).Activationofthesupplementarymotorareaandofattentionalnetworksduringtemporalprocessing.Exp.BrainRes.142,475–485.doi:10.1007/s00221-001-0953-0 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Macefield,V.G.(2009).“TactileCfibers,”inEncyclopediaofNeuroscience,edsM.D.Binder,N.HirokawaandU.Windhorst(Germany:Springer),3995–3998. GoogleScholar McGlone,F.,Wessberg,J.,andOlausson,H.(2014).Discriminativeandaffectivetouch:sensingandfeeling.Neuron82,737–755.doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.001 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Mella,N.,Conty,L.,andPouthas,V.(2011).Theroleofphysiologicalarousalintimeperception:psychophysiologicalevidencefromanemotionregulationparadigm.BrainCogn.75,182–187.doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.012 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Mioni,G.,Stablum,F.,Prunetti,E.,andGrondin,S.(2016).Timeperceptioninanxiousanddepressedpatients:acomparisonbetweentimereproductionandtimeproductiontasks.J.Affect.Disord.196,154–163.doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.047 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Naqvi,N.H.,Gaznick,N.,Tranel,D.,andBechara,A.(2014).Theinsula:acriticalneuralsubstrateforcravinganddrugseekingunderconflictandrisk.Ann.NYAcad.Sci.1316,53–70.doi:10.1111/nyas.12415 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Nordin,M.(1990).Low-thresholdmechanoreceptiveandnociceptiveunitswithunmyelinated(C)fibresinthehumansupraorbitalnerve.J.Physiol.426,229–240.doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018135 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Ogden,R.S.(2013).Theeffectoffacialattractivenessontemporalperception.Cogn.Emot.27,1292–1304.doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.769426 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Ogden,R.S.,Moore,D.,Redfern,L.,andMcGlone,F.(2015a).Theeffectofpainandtheanticipationofpainontemporalperception:aroleforattentionandarousal.Cogn.Emot.29,910–922.doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.954529 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Ogden,R.S.,Moore,D.,Redfern,L.,andMcGlone,F.(2015b).Strokemeforlongerthistouchfeelstooshort:theeffectofpleasanttouchontemporalperception.Conscious.Cogn.36,306–313.doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.006 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Olausson,H.,Lamarre,Y.,Backlund,H.,Morin,C.,Wallin,B.G.,Starck,G.,etal.(2002).Unmyelinatedtactileafferentssignaltouchandprojecttoinsularcortex.Nat.Neurosci.5,900–904.doi:10.1038/nn896 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Olausson,H.,Wessberg,J.,andMcGlone,F.(2016).AffectiveTouchandtheNeurophysiologyofCTAfferents.NewYork,NY:Springer. GoogleScholar Ondobaka,S.,Kilner,J.,andFriston,K.(2017).Theroleofinteroceptiveinferenceintheoryofmind.BrainCogn.112,64–68.doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.08.002 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Orgs,G.,Kirsch,L.,andHaggard,P.(2013).Timeperceptionduringapparentbiologicalmotionreflectssubjectivespeedofmovement,notobjectiverateofvisualstimulation.Exp.BrainRes.227,223–229.doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3502-8 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Paulus,M.P.,andStein,M.B.(2010).Interoceptioninanxietyanddepression.BrainStruct.Funct.214,451–463.doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0258-9 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pezzulo,G.(2014).Whydoyoufearthebogeyman?Anembodiedpredictivecodingmodelofperceptualinference.Cogn.Affect.Behav.Neurosci.14,902–911.doi:10.3758/s13415-013-0227-x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pezzulo,G.,Iodice,P.,Barca,L.,Chausse,P.,Monceau,S.,andMermillod,M.(2018).Increasedheartrateafterexercisefacilitatestheprocessingoffearfulbutnotdisgustedfaces.Sci.Rep.8:398.doi:10.1038/s41598-017-18761-5 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pezzulo,G.,Rigoli,F.,andFriston,K.(2015).Activeinference,homeostaticregulationandadaptivebehaviouralcontrol.Prog.Neurobiol.134,17–35.doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.001 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pollatos,O.,Kurz,A.L.,Albrecht,J.,Schreder,T.,Kleemann,A.M.,Schöpf,V.,etal.(2008).Reducedperceptionofbodilysignalsinanorexianervosa.Eat.Behav.9,381–388.doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.02.001 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pollatos,O.,Laubrock,J.,andWittmann,M.(2014).Interoceptivefocusshapestheexperienceoftime.PLoSOne9:e86934.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086934 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pollatos,O.,Schandry,R.,Auer,D.P.,andKaufmann,C.(2007a).Brainstructuresmediatingcardiovasculararousalandinteroceptiveawareness.BrainRes.1141,178–187.doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.026 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pollatos,O.,Traut-Mattausch,E.,Schroeder,H.,andSchandry,R.(2007b).Interoceptiveawarenessmediatestherelationshipbetweenanxietyandtheintensityofunpleasantfeelings.J.AnxietyDisord.21,931–943.doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.004 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Pollatos,O.,Traut-Mattausch,E.,andSchandry,R.(2009).Differentialeffectsofanxietyanddepressiononinteroceptiveaccuracy.Depress.Anxiety26,167–173.doi:10.1002/da.20504 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Rao,S.M.,Mayer,A.R.,andHarrington,D.L.(2001).Theevolutionofbrainactivationduringtemporalprocessing.Nat.Neurosci.4,317–323.doi:10.1038/85191 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Ring,C.,Brener,J.,Knapp,K.,andMailloux,J.(2015).Effectsofheartbeatfeedbackonbeliefsaboutheartrateandheartbeatcounting:acautionarytaleaboutinteroceptiveawareness.Biol.Psychol.104,193–198.doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.010 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Riva,G.,Serino,S.,DiLernia,D.,Pavone,E.F.,andDakanalis,A.(2017).Embodiedmedicine:menssanaincorporevirtualesano.Front.Hum.Neurosci.11:120.doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00120 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Riva,G.,Villani,D.,Cipresso,P.,Repetto,C.,Triberti,S.,DiLernia,D.,etal.(2016).Positiveandtransformativetechnologiesforactiveageing.Stud.HealthTechnol.Inform.220,308–315.doi:10.3233/978-1-61499-625-5-308 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Rosso,I.M.,Makris,N.,Britton,J.C.,Price,L.M.,Gold,A.L.,Zai,D.,etal.(2010).Anxietysensitivitycorrelateswithtwoindicesofrightanteriorinsulastructureinspecificanimalphobia.Depress.Anxiety27,1104–1110.doi:10.1002/da.20765 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Roudaut,Y.,Lonigro,A.,Coste,B.,Hao,J.,Delmas,P.,andCrest,M.(2012).Touchsense:functionalorganizationandmoleculardeterminantsofmechanosensitivereceptors.Channels6,234–245.doi:10.4161/chan.22213 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Schaefer,M.,Egloff,B.,andWitthöft,M.(2012).Isinteroceptiveawarenessreallyalteredinsomatoformdisorders?Testingcompetingtheorieswithtwoparadigmsofheartbeatperception.J.Abnorm.Psychol.121,719–724.doi:10.1037/a0028509 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Schandry,R.(1981).Heartbeatperceptionandemotionalexperience.Psychophysiology18,483–488.doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Seth,A.K.(2013).Interoceptiveinference,emotionandtheembodiedself.TrendsCogn.Sci.17,565–573.doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Seth,A.K.,Suzuki,K.,andCritchley,H.D.(2011).Aninteroceptivepredictivecodingmodelofconsciouspresence.Front.Psychol.2:395.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Ševcík,J.,Kainz,O.,Fecilak,P.,andJakab,F.(2015).SystemforEKGmonitoring.Int.J.Adv.Res.Artif.Intell.4,doi:10.14569/IJARAI.2015.040905 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Sliz,D.,andHayley,S.(2012).Majordepressivedisorderandalterationsininsularcorticalactivity:areviewofcurrentfunctionalmagneticimagingresearch.Front.Hum.Neurosci.6:323.doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00323 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Spielberger,C.D.,Gorsuch,R.L.,andLushene,R.E.(1970).ManualforTheState-TraitAnxietyInventory.PaloAlto,CA:ConsultingPsychologistsPress. GoogleScholar Spitoni,G.F.,Serino,A.,Cotugno,A.,Mancini,F.,Antonucci,G.,andPizzamiglio,L.(2015).ThetwodimensionsofthebodyrepresentationinwomensufferingfromAnorexiaNervosa.PsychiatryRes.230,181–188.doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.036 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Sprengelmeyer,R.,Steele,J.D.,Mwangi,B.,Kumar,P.,Christmas,D.,Milders,M.,etal.(2011).Theinsularcortexandtheneuroanatomyofmajordepression.J.Affect.Disord.133,120–127.doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.04.004 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Stanton,T.R.,Lin,C.W.,Bray,H.,Smeets,R.J.,Taylor,D.,Law,R.Y.,etal.(2013).Tactileacuityisdisruptedinosteoarthritisbutisunrelatedtodisruptionsinmotorimageryperformance.Rheumatology52,1509–1519.doi:10.1093/rheumatology/ket139 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Steer,R.A.,Ball,R.,Ranieri,W.F.,andBeck,A.T.(1999).Dimensionsofthebeckdepressioninventory-IIinclinicallydepressedoutpatients.J.Clin.Psychol.55,117–128.doi:10.1002/(sici)1097-4679(199901)55:1<117::aid-jclp12>3.0.co;2-a PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Sterling,P.(2012).Allostasis:amodelofpredictiveregulation.Physiol.Behav.106,5–15.doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Stojanović,R.,Čaplánová,A.,Kovačević,Ž.,Nemanja,N.,andBundalo,Z.(2015).Alternativeapproachtoaddressinginfrastructureneedsinbiomedicalengineeringprograms(Caseofemergingeconomies).FoliaMed.Fac.Med.Univ.Sarav.50,29–33. GoogleScholar Storch,E.A.,Roberti,J.W.,andRoth,D.A.(2004).Factorstructure,concurrentvalidity,andinternalconsistencyoftheBeckDepressionInventory-SecondEditioninasampleofcollegestudents.Depress.Anxiety19,187–189.doi:10.1002/da.20002 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Stratmann,M.,Konrad,C.,Kugel,H.,Krug,A.,Schöning,S.,Ohrmann,P.,etal.(2014).Insularandhippocampalgraymattervolumereductionsinpatientswithmajordepressivedisorder.PLoSOne9:e102692.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102692 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Suzuki,K.,Garfinkel,S.N.,Critchley,H.D.,andSeth,A.K.(2013).Multisensoryintegrationacrossexteroceptiveandinteroceptivedomainsmodulatesself-experienceintherubber-handillusion.Neuropsychologia51,2909–2917.doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.014 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Thones,S.,andOberfeld,D.(2015).Timeperceptionindepression:ameta-analysis.J.Affect.Disord.175,359–372.doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.057 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Tomassini,A.,Gori,M.,Burr,D.,Sandini,G.,andMorrone,C.(2011).Perceiveddurationofvisualandtactilestimulidependsonperceivedspeed.Front.Integr.Neurosci.5:51.doi:10.3389/fnint.2011.00051 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Tomassini,A.,andMorrone,M.C.(2016).Perceivedvisualtimedependsonmotorpreparationanddirectionofhandmovements.Sci.Rep.6:27947.doi:10.1038/srep27947 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Treisman,M.,Faulkner,A.,Naish,P.L.,andBrogan,D.(1990).Theinternalclock:evidenceforatemporaloscillatorunderlyingtimeperceptionwithsomeestimatesofitscharacteristicfrequency.Perception19,705–742.doi:10.1068/p190705 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Tsakiris,M.,Tajadura-Jiménez,A.,andCostantini,M.(2011).Justaheartbeatawayfromone’sbody:interoceptivesensitivitypredictsmalleabilityofbody-representations.Proc.Biol.Sci.278,2470–2476.doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.2547 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Tse,P.U.,Intriligator,J.,Rivest,J.,andCavanagh,P.(2004).Attentionandthesubjectiveexpansionoftime.Percept.Psychophys.66,1171–1189.doi:10.3758/bf03196844 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Uddin,L.Q.(2015).Salienceprocessingandinsularcorticalfunctionanddysfunction.Nat.Rev.Neurosci.16,55–61.doi:10.1038/nrn3857 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Vallbo,A.B.,Olausson,H.,andWessberg,J.(1999).Unmyelinatedafferentsconstituteasecondsystemcodingtactilestimuliofthehumanhairyskin.J.Neurophysiol.81,2753–2763.doi:10.1152/jn.1999.81.6.2753 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Vallbo,A.B.,Olausson,H.,Wessberg,J.,andKakuda,N.(1995).Receptivefieldcharacteristicsoftactileunitswithmyelinatedafferentsinhairyskinofhumansubjects.J.Physiol.483,783–795.doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020622 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar vanWassenhove,V.,Buonomano,D.V.,Shimojo,S.,andShams,L.(2008).Distortionsofsubjectivetimeperceptionwithinandacrosssenses.PLoSOne3:e1437.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001437 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Vigneau,F.,andCormier,S.(2008).ThefactorstructureoftheState-TraitAnxietyInventory:analternativeview.J.Pers.Assess.90,280–285.doi:10.1080/00223890701885027 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Villarrubia,G.,DePaz,J.F.,Bajo,J.,andCorchado,J.M.(2014a).EKGmobile.Adv.Sci.Technol.Lett.49,95–100.doi:10.14257/astl.2014.49.20 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Villarrubia,G.,DePaz,J.F.,Corchado,J.M.,andBajo,J.(2014b).“EKGintelligentmobilesystemforhomeusers,”inAdvancesinArtificialIntelligence—IBERAMIA2014.IBERAMIA2014,edsA.BazzanandK.Pichara,(Cham:Springer)8864,767–778.doi:10.1007/978-3-319-12027-0_62LectureNotesinComputerScience. CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Watson,D.,Weber,K.,Assenheimer,J.S.,Clark,L.A.,Strauss,M.E.,andMcCormick,R.A.(1995).Testingatripartitemodel:I.Evaluatingtheconvergentanddiscriminantvalidityofanxietyanddepressionsymptomscales.J.Abnorm.Psychol.104,3–14.doi:10.1037/0021-843x.104.1.3 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wearden,J.H.,Edwards,H.,Fakhri,M.,andPercival,A.(1998).Why“soundsarejudgedlongerthanlights”:applicationofamodeloftheinternalclockinhumans.Q.J.Exp.Psychol.B51,97–120. PubMedAbstract|GoogleScholar Wearden,J.,O’Rourke,S.C.,Matchwick,C.,Min,Z.,andMaeers,S.(2010).Taskswitchingandsubjectiveduration.Q.J.Exp.Psychol.63,531–543.doi:10.1080/17470210903024768 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wearden,J.H.,andPenton-Voak,I.S.(1995).Feelingtheheat:bodytemperatureandtherateofsubjectivetime,revisited.Q.J.Exp.Psychol.B48,129–141. PubMedAbstract|GoogleScholar Weiss,S.,Sack,M.,Henningsen,P.,andPollatos,O.(2014).Ontheinteractionofself-regulation,interoceptionandpainperception.Psychopathology47,377–382.doi:10.1159/000365107 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wessberg,J.,Olausson,H.,Fernström,K.W.,andVallbo,A.B.(2003).Receptivefieldpropertiesofunmyelinatedtactileafferentsinthehumanskin.J.Neurophysiol.89,1567–1575.doi:10.1152/jn.00256.2002 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Whyman,A.D.,andMoos,R.H.(1967).Timeperceptionandanxiety.Percept.Mot.Skills24,567–570.doi:10.2466/pms.1967.24.2.567 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wiebking,C.,deGreck,M.,Duncan,N.W.,Tempelmann,C.,Bajbouj,M.,andNorthoff,G.(2015).Interoceptionininsulasubregionsasapossiblestatemarkerfordepression-anexploratoryfMRIstudyinvestigatinghealthy,depressedandremittedparticipants.Front.Behav.Neurosci.9:82.doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00082 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wiech,K.,Lin,C.S.,Brodersen,K.H.,Bingel,U.,Ploner,M.,andTracey,I.(2010).Anteriorinsulaintegratesinformationaboutsalienceintoperceptualdecisionsaboutpain.J.Neurosci.30,16324–16331.doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2087-10.2010 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wierenga,C.E.,Bischoff-Grethe,A.,Melrose,A.J.,Irvine,Z.,Torres,L.,Bailer,U.F.,etal.(2015).Hungerdoesnotmotivaterewardinwomenremittedfromanorexianervosa.Biol.Psychiatry77,642–652.doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.024 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wittmann,M.(2009).Theinnerexperienceoftime.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.BBiol.Sci.364,1955–1967.doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0003 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wittmann,M.(2014).“Embodiedtime:theexperienceoftime,thebody,andtheself,”inSubjectiveTime:ThePhilosophy,Psychology,andNeuroscienceofTemporality,edsV.ArstilaandD.Lloyd(Cambridge,MA:MITpress),507–523. GoogleScholar Wittmann,M.,andPaulus,M.P.(2008).Decisionmaking,impulsivityandtimeperception.TrendsCogn.Sci.12,7–12.doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wittmann,M.,Simmons,A.N.,Aron,J.L.,andPaulus,M.P.(2010).Accumulationofneuralactivityintheposteriorinsulaencodesthepassageoftime.Neuropsychologia48,3110–3120.doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.023 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wittmann,M.,andvanWassenhove,V.(2009).Theexperienceoftime:neuralmechanismsandtheinterplayofemotion,cognitionandembodiment.Philos.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.BBiol.Sci.364,1809–1813.doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0025 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Wylie,K.P.,andTregellas,J.R.(2010).Theroleoftheinsulainschizophrenia.Schizophr.Res.123,93–104.doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.08.027 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Yoo,J.Y.,andLee,J.H.(2015).Theeffectsofvalenceandarousalontimeperceptioninindividualswithsocialanxiety.Front.Psychol.6:1208.doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01208 PubMedAbstract|CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Zakay,D.,andBlock,R.A.(1996).Theroleofattentionintimeestimationprocesses.Adv.Psychol.115,143–164.doi:10.1016/s0166-4115(96)80057-4 CrossRefFullText|GoogleScholar Keywords:interoception,interoceptivebuffer,C-Tfibers,timeperception Citation:DiLerniaD,SerinoS,PezzuloG,PedroliE,CipressoPandRivaG(2018)FeeltheTime.TimePerceptionasaFunctionofInteroceptiveProcessing.Front.Hum.Neurosci.12:74.doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00074 Received:28October2017;Accepted:12February2018;Published:06March2018. Editedby:IrinaStrigo,UniversityofCalifornia,SanFrancisco,UnitedStates Reviewedby:MarcWittmann,InstitutfürGrenzgebietederPsychologieundPsychohygiene,GermanyJ.MichaelHerrmann,UniversityofEdinburgh,UnitedKingdom Copyright©2018DiLernia,Serino,Pezzulo,Pedroli,CipressoandRiva.Thisisanopen-accessarticledistributedunderthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionLicense(CCBY).Theuse,distributionorreproductioninotherforumsispermitted,providedtheoriginalauthor(s)andthecopyrightownerarecreditedandthattheoriginalpublicationinthisjournaliscited,inaccordancewithacceptedacademicpractice.Nouse,distributionorreproductionispermittedwhichdoesnotcomplywiththeseterms. *Correspondence:DanieleDiLernia,[email protected] COMMENTARY ORIGINALARTICLE Peoplealsolookedat SuggestaResearchTopic>
延伸文章資訊
- 1Time perception - Wikipedia
The study of time perception or chronoception is a field within psychology, cognitive linguistics...
- 2Time perception: the bad news and the good - PMC - NCBI
The perception of time is fundamental to our experience and central to virtually all of our activ...
- 3Timing & Time Perception | Brill
Timing & Time Perception ... Timing is ever-present in our everyday life – from the ringing sound...
- 4Time Perception
Time perception refers to a person's subjective experience of the passage of time, or the perceiv...
- 5The inner experience of time - PMC - NCBI
Cognitive and neural models of time perception